Skip to main content

Financial-related discrimination and socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being related measures: a longitudinal study

Abstract

Background

This study examined the prospective association between financial-related discrimination and psychological well-being related measures and assessed the role of financial-related discrimination in explaining socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being related measures.

Methods

Data of UK older adults (≥ 50 years) from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing were used (baseline: Wave 5, 2010/2011; n = 8,988). The baseline total non-pension wealth (in tertiles: poorest, middle, richest) was used as a socioeconomic status (SES) measure. Financial-related discrimination at baseline was defined as participants who reported they had been discriminated against due to their financial status. Five psychological well-being related measures (depressive symptoms, enjoyment of life, eudemonic well-being, life satisfaction and loneliness) were examined prospectively across different follow-up periods (Waves 6, 2012/2013, 2-year follow-up; and 7, 2014/2015, 4-year follow-up). Regression models assessed associations between wealth, financial-related discrimination, and follow-up psychological measures, controlling for sociodemographic covariates and baseline psychological measures (for longitudinal associations). Mediation analysis informed how much (%) the association between wealth and psychological well-being related measures was explained by financial-related discrimination.

Results

Participants from the poorest, but not middle, (vs. richest) wealth groups were more likely to experience financial-related discrimination (OR = 1.97; 95%CI = 1.49, 2.59). The poorest (vs. richest) wealth was also longitudinally associated with increased depressive symptoms and decreased enjoyment of life, eudemonic well-being and life satisfaction in both 2-year and 4-year follow-ups, and increased loneliness at 4-year follow-up. Experiencing financial-related discrimination was longitudinally associated with greater depressive symptoms and loneliness, and lower enjoyment of life across follow-up periods. Findings from mediation analysis indicated that financial-related discrimination explained 3–8% of the longitudinal associations between wealth (poorest vs. richest) and psychological well-being related measures.

Conclusions

Financial-related discrimination is associated with worse psychological well-being and explains a small proportion of socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Globally, the prevalence of mental health problems (e.g., depressive, anxiety disorders) increased by 48% between 1990 and 2019 [1], and this has become a major public health problem [2]. Furthermore, socioeconomic inequalities in mental health, including psychological factors related to well-being, have widened. Across multiple British population surveys, the prevalence of psychological distress in the most deprived areas was estimated to be double that of the least deprived areas [3]. In line with this, a meta-analysis indicated significant socioeconomic inequalities in depression, whereby individuals from a range of lower socioeconomic status (SES) indicators (e.g., education, income), had higher odds of being depressed than their higher SES counterparts [4]. These SES-based inequalities are consistent across a range of psychological well-being related measures, including enjoyment of life [5], life satisfaction [6], and loneliness [7]. Therefore, further understanding socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being is vital to inform targeted public health interventions aimed at reducing these inequalities.

A previous study used data from the UK Whitehall II Study and found that social support, material disadvantage, work characteristics, and health-related behaviors, may explain SES-related employment gradients observed for depressive symptoms [8]. Likewise, a national study of elderly Chinese adults showed that social participation, via group exercise, mediated 20% of the association between lower income and worse mental health [9]. These findings support KA Matthews and LC Gallo [10]’s proposition that stress and lack of social support in part may explain why low SES and poor health are related. Research has since demonstrated that income inequality (i.e., uneven income distribution across a population) moderates the negative association between relative income (i.e., an individual’s income compared to others) and life satisfaction [11]. This implies that low SES individuals may have worse psychological well-being due to social comparison of income, which evokes feelings of unfairness and lack of trust [12].

Another possible explanation for socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being is stigma, defined by E Goffman [13] as the “deeply discrediting attributes” possessed by an individual which exclude them from full social acceptance (pp. 3–4). Stigma is linked to decreased psychological well-being through a convergence of labelling, stereotyping, separation, status loss and discrimination, which occur within power imbalanced contexts [14]. Experiencing stigma or discrimination has been identified as a source of psychological distress in people from lower SES backgrounds, positing SES-based discrimination as a potential mechanism for explaining SES-based inequalities in psychological well-being [15]. A longitudinal cohort study from the Midlife in the United States study (MIDUS) revealed that between 1990 and 2010, the prevalence of, and gaps in daily discrimination received between the highest and lowest SES groups both increased [16]. Using the same dataset, perceived inequality in work and everyday discrimination explained 22% of longitudinal associations between socioeconomic disadvantage and self-rated health [17]. A study from the US showed that amongst women in poverty, internalised stigma (an individual’s internal negative feelings about poverty), and experienced stigma (an individual’s perception of being stigmatized by others) were significant and distinct factors associated with depression [18].

However, there is a dearth of evidence quantifying the extent to which discrimination, specifically attributed to lack of financial resources, explains socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being. Recently, A Amirova, KA Rimes and RA Hackett [19] assessed the prevalence of perceived financial-related discrimination in UK and US adults. Perceived financial-related discrimination was defined as any individual who reported experiencing at least one of five discriminative situations (e.g., being threatened or harassed) and then attributed this to their financial status when asked the reason(s) why they believe they were discriminated against. Participants of lower SES were more likely to report experiencing financial-related discrimination compared to higher SES participants, although the study did not assess to the extent to which financial-related discrimination was associated with psychological well-being or explained socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being [19].

Compared to generalised or overt stigma and discrimination (e.g., race, weight), financial-related discrimination may be most relevant for understanding socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being, given its likely close relation to SES. Therefore, this study aimed to examine if financial-related discrimination increased prospective risk of poorer psychological well-being, and in part explained the development of socioeconomic inequalities across a range of psychological well-being measures. We focused on English older adults from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), as the only UK longitudinal study to date which has collected information on financial-related discrimination. ELSA also has a diverse range of psychological well-being measures. We hypothesised that perceived financial-related discrimination would be associated with poorer psychological well-being over time, and that it would partly mediate, or explain, prospective associations between wealth (as a measure of SES) and psychological well-being related measures.

Methods

Participants

ELSA is an ongoing prospective cohort study initiated in 2002, comprised of 12,099 adults aged ≥ 50 from households who completed the Health Survey for England (HSE) between 1998 and 2001. ELSA participants are interviewed every two years to measure changes in health, social and economic circumstances [20]. This study used data from waves 5 (2010/2011), 6 (2012/2013), and 7 (2014/2015) [21]. Wave 5 was selected as baseline because a range of psychological well-being measures were collected, and this is the only wave to date which measured perceived discrimination, including financial-related discrimination [20]. Using baseline wealth as a SES measure, development of socioeconomic inequalities in a diverse range of psychological well-being measures at Waves 6 (2-year follow-up) and 7 (4-year follow-up) were assessed, to determine the consistency of associations. A total of 10,274 participants contributed data to Wave 5, 9,090 of these being ‘core’ participants who met age eligibility criteria. For the analysis, we included sample weights at baseline (Wave 5) to adjust for non-response. Therefore, we excluded participants without sample weights, resulting in the maximum analytical sample size of 8,988. ELSA has received ethical approval from different institutional review boards for its waves (https://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/ethical-approval). Informed consent was sought from all the ELSA participants.

Independent variable: Wealth

We used wealth at baseline (Wave 5) as a SES measure in this study. Wealth in ELSA was determined by the total non-pension wealth, including financial wealth from savings and investments, value of home, property, business, and physical wealth assets (e.g., artwork) owned by the respondent/responding couple and dependants, minus any debt [22, 23]. We transformed total non-pension wealth calculated and available in ELSA into tertiles (poorest, middle, richest) (e.g., as in [22]). Table S1 presents distribution of total wealth by tertile. The top tertile (richest) had the biggest range (minimum– maximum values) compared to the lowest (poorest) and middle tertiles.

Even though other SES measures were available (e.g., education, employment status), we examined wealth only, because it was found to be the most robust SES indicator in ELSA, and has the strongest association with mortality in older age [22, 24]. When studying older adults who may be retired or unemployed and not receive income anymore, employment status and income level tend to be less relevant. Therefore, wealth may serve as a better measure, as it captures financial and other resources at older ages more accurately than other traditional SES measures (e.g., income) [23, 25].

Candidate mediator: Financial-related discrimination

During Wave 5, participants completed an adapted version of the Perceived Everyday Experiences With Discrimination Scale [26], containing five items assessing the frequency and context, of five forms of discriminative treatment: “In your day-to-day life, how often have any of the following things happened to you (1) you are treated with less respect or courtesy; (2) you receive poorer service than other people in restaurants and shops; (3) people act as if they think you are not clever; (4) you are threatened or harassed; (5) you receive poorer service or treatment than other people from doctors or hospitals”. Participants then rated each item on a 7-point scale (1= “almost every day”, 7= “never”). Participants who responded, “almost every day”, “at least once a week”, “a few times a year” or “less than once a year” to any of the five forms of discriminative treatment were then asked to select the reason(s) why they were discriminated against, including “financial status”. Consistent with previous literature examining the association between perceived discrimination and health outcomes [19, 27], financial-related discrimination was defined as participants who reported that their “financial status” was the reason why they were discriminated against. In the current study, whether a participant had experienced financial-related discrimination or not, based on the above criteria, was then dichotomised into “yes” or “no” groups.

Dependent variables: psychological well-being related measures

Five different psychological well-being related measures at Waves 6 and 7: depressive symptoms, enjoyment of life, eudemonic well-being, life satisfaction and loneliness, were examined, to provide a holistic overview of how psychological well-being related measures may vary according to wealth and perceived financial-related discrimination.

Depressive symptoms were measured using the short-form Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [28], containing eight items assessing participants feelings over the last week, to which participants responded “yes” or “no”. Negative items (e.g., “You feel depressed?”) were coded as: 0= “no” and 1= “yes”, whereas positively worded items (e.g., “You were happy?”) were reverse coded: 0= “yes” and 1= “no”. This produced a total sum score ranging from 0 to 8, with higher scores indicating greater depressive symptoms (e.g., as in [29]).

Enjoyment of life was measured using four items (e.g., “I enjoy the things that I do,” “I enjoy being in the company of others”), from the Control, Autonomy, Self-Realization and Pleasure (CASP-19) Quality of Life instrument [30]. Participants responded to items on a 4-point scale (0= “never”, 3= “often”), with higher total scores, ranging from 0 to 12, indicating greater enjoyment of life (e.g., as in [31]).

Eudemonic well-being was assessed using the remaining 15 items (e.g., “I feel free to plan for the future”) from the CASP-19 instrument [30]. Similar to the scoring of enjoyment of life, participants responded to each item on a 4-point Likert scale (0= “never”, 3= “often”), with responses to negative items (e.g., “I feel that what happens to me is out of my control”) reverse coded. A total score ranging from 0 to 45 was generated by adding all the items together, with higher scores indicating greater eudemonic well-being (e.g., as in [31]).

Life satisfaction was measured using the Satisfaction with Life scale [32], which contained five items (e.g., “In most ways my life is close to ideal”), which participants responded to on a 7-point Likert scale (1= “strongly agree”, 7= “strongly disagree”). Higher total scores, ranging from 1 to 35, indicated increased life satisfaction (e.g., as in [33]).

Loneliness was quantified using the University of California Los Angeles (ULCA)3-item loneliness scale [34], where participants responded to items (e.g., “How often do you feel you lack companionship?”) with 1= “hardly ever or never”, 2= “some of the time” or 3= “often”. Higher total scores, ranging from 1 to 9, indicated greater loneliness (e.g., as in [35]).

Covariates

Age (in years), sex (female; male), ethnicity (White; non-White), marital status (single/never married; married; divorced/separated/widowed), education level (degree or equivalent; non-degree) (e.g., as in [31]), employment status (employed; self-employed; retired/semi-retired; unemployed/other) (e.g., as in [36]) and limiting illness status (yes; no), were selected as covariates, as these have been shown to influence psychological well-being and/or perceived discrimination [17, 37].

Data analysis

Primary analyses

Data analysis was conducted using STATA. We included sample weights available at Wave 5 in the analyses to take into account differences in baseline characteristics associated with non-response. Descriptive statistics for categorical variables (sex, ethnicity, marital status, education, employment status, wealth, limiting illness status and financial-related discrimination) were expressed as frequencies and weighted percentages, whereas continuous variables (age) and psychological well-being related measures were expressed as weighted mean and standard deviation (SD). For regression and mediation analyses, psychological well-being related measures across waves were transformed into z-scores (mean = 0; SD = 1), to enable comparison across different matrices.

Cross-sectional associations between wealth and financial-related discrimination at baseline (wave 5) were tested using logistic regression, controlling for baseline sociodemographic covariates. Results were expressed as odds ratio (OR), 95% confidence intervals (CI), and p-values. Linear regressions were then used to examine longitudinal associations between baseline wealth (Wave 5) and psychological well-being related measures at follow-up (Waves 6 and 7), controlling for baseline psychological well-being and sociodemographic covariates (Wave 5). Next, longitudinal associations between financial-related discrimination at baseline (Wave 5) and each psychological well-being related measure at different follow-ups (Waves 6 and 7) were investigated using linear regression, adjusted for baseline psychological well-being and sociodemographic covariates. Results were reported as regression coefficients (β), 95% CI, and p-values.

Mediation analyses were conducted to examine the role of financial-related discrimination in explaining the association between baseline wealth and follow-up psychological well-being related measures. To ensure mediation preconditions were satisfied [38], the following three associations had to be significant: (1) association between wealth (independent variable– IV) and financial-related discrimination (mediator– M) (path a), (2) association between wealth (IV) and psychological well-being (dependent variable– DV) (total effect, path c), and (3) association between financial-related discrimination (M) and psychological well-being (DV) (path b) (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mediation model. Path a depicts cross-sectional associations between wealth and financial-related discrimination, controlling for baseline psychological well-being related measures and sociodemographic covariates (age, sex, ethnicity, marital status, education, employment status, limiting illness status). Path b illustrates longitudinal associations between financial-related discrimination and psychological well-being related measures at 2-year and 4-year follow-ups, controlling for baseline psychological well-being and sociodemographic covariates. Path c’ represents the direct effect of wealth on psychological well-being related measures longitudinally, in an unmediated model. The indirect effect (path a × b) estimates the longitudinal effect of wealth on psychological well-being related measures via financial-related discrimination. Path c describes the total effect ((a × b) + c’), which estimates the sum of both the indirect and direct effects

Having satisfied mediation preconditions, single mediation models using the Karlson-Holm-Breen (KHB) method [39], were used to examine the mediating role of financial-related discrimination. Within the mediation model, wealth (IV) and financial-related discrimination (M) were both measured at baseline (Wave 5), instead of measuring financial-related discrimination at a separate timepoint after baseline. This was chosen to reduce the possibility that changes in wealth between (baseline– time 1) and immediate wave after baseline (time 2), influence financial-related discrimination, which may then have masked the true effect of baseline wealth on financial-related discrimination. This becomes especially relevant when considering the dynamic nature of wealth variables in older populations, given the unexpected changes to wealth that arise with ageing (e.g., unexpected healthcare costs, costs of managing a long-term health condition) [40]. Our approach for mediation analysis, whereby the independent variable and candidate mediator were measured at the same time point (baseline), was also used by previous studies [31, 41, 42].Mediation findings were presented as total effect (path c = (a × b) + c’): combined direct and indirect effect), direct effect (path c’), and indirect effect (path a × b), along with 95% CI and p-values. Effect ratios (indirect effect/total effect), indicating how much the association between wealth index and psychological well-being was explained by financial-related discrimination, were also presented.

As missing values observed in the majority of variables may introduce selection bias, we used multiple imputation by chained equation (MICE) [43, 44] to address missing values in this study. We assumed missing at random as some variables, including age, ethnicity, and marital status, were associated with missingness. We used ‘mi impute chained’ in STATA to create 20 imputed datasets to fill in missing values. We included all variables (independent, mediator, dependent variables) in the imputation model and treated variables with no missing values (age, sex, marital status, see Table 1) as predictors (e.g., as in [45]). Following previous guidelines [46], we also included baseline sample weights to improve the imputation model. Using MICE, the final sample size is the same as the maximum analytical sample size (n = 8,988).

Table 1 Baseline sociodemographic characteristics, financial related-stigma, and psychological well-being related measures

Additional/sensitivity analyses

Findings from previous studies indicated that prior health status may be associated with current material factors (e.g., wealth) [47, 48]. To take into account the influence of earlier psychological well-being on current wealth at baseline, we conducted additional analyses by controlling for pre-baseline psychological well-being at Wave 4 (as opposed to control for baseline psychological well-being at Wave 5) in regression and mediation analyses predicting follow-up psychological well-being (Waves 6, 7).

Results

Baseline participant characteristics

Table 1 presents that a roughly equivalent sample of male and female participants were included (47% and 53%, respectively), aged 67.26 years on average. Participants were predominantly White (96%), retired (56%), did not hold a degree level qualification (85%), were married or cohabiting (69%) and did not have a limiting illness (64%). Participants who reported experiencing discrimination attributed to financial-related discrimination was 6%. The proportions of financial-related discrimination were higher amongst participants from the poorest (9%), than those from middle (5%) and richest (4%) wealth groups (Table S2). Participants had favourable psychological well-being at baseline, as the total scores for depressive symptoms and loneliness were lower, and total scores of enjoyment of life, eudemonic well-being, and life satisfaction were higher than 50% of their respective possible total scores (Table 1). This is similar to follow-up psychological well-being (Table S3).

Associations between wealth and financial-related discrimination

Table 2 depicts adjusted cross-sectional associations between wealth and financial-related discrimination. Participants from the poorest (vs. richest) wealth group were more likely to experience financial-related discrimination (OR = 1.97; 95% CI = 1.49, 2.59). There was no significant difference in the likelihood of experiencing financial-related discrimination between the middle and richest participants (OR = 1.16; 95% CI = 0.86, 1.55). However, compared to the middle group, participants from the poorest group were also more likely to experience financial-related discrimination (OR = 1.70; 95% CI = 1.32, 2.20) (findings are not presented in Table 2). For other possible SES measures (e.g., education), we found no strong evidence of the association between these SES measures and financial-related discrimination. Due to larger effect size for the comparison between the poorest vs. richest groups (OR = 1.97) than the poorest vs. middle groups (OR = 1.70), our mediation analysis focused on the poorest vs. richest groups. In addition, the associations between wealth and psychological well-being related measures were presented based on this comparison (the poorest vs. richest).

Table 2 Adjusted cross-sectional associations between wealth and financial-related discrimination (n = 8,988)

Associations between wealth and psychological well-being related measures

Table 3 depicts longitudinal associations between wealth (poorest vs. richest) and psychological well-being related measures at Waves 6 and 7, adjusted for baseline psychological well-being and sociodemographic covariates. Participants from the poorest (vs. richest) group consistently had greater depressive symptoms and lower enjoyment of life, eudemonic wellbeing, and life satisfaction at both 2-year and 4-year follow-up. There were also significant differences in loneliness between the poorest and richest participants for 4-year follow-up only.

Table 3 Longitudinal associations between wealth (poorest vs. richest) and psychological well-being related measures (n = 8,988)

Associations between financial-related discrimination and psychological well-being related measures

Experiencing financial-related discrimination was associated with increased depressive symptoms at 2-year (β = 0.17, 95% CI: 0.07, 0.27) and 4-year follow-ups (β = 0.22, 95% CI: 0.11, 0.33), decreased enjoyment of life at 2-year (β = -0.16, 95% CI: -0.26, -0.07) and 4 year-follow ups (β = -0.12, 95% CI: -0.22, -0.02), and increased loneliness at 2-year (β = 0.12, 95% CI: 0.03, 0.20) and 4-year follow-ups (β = 0.14, 95% CI: 0.04, 0.23) (Table 4). Financial-related discrimination was not associated with eudemonic wellbeing and life satisfaction at both follow-ups. These findings supported our hypothesis that financial-related discrimination may be prospectively associated with worsening of psychological well-being. As per the preconditions for mediation, only depressive symptoms, and enjoyment of life at both follow-ups, and loneliness at 4-year follow-up, were included in subsequent mediation analyses.

Table 4 Longitudinal associations between financial-related discrimination (yes vs. no) and psychological well-being related measures (n = 8,988)

Mediation by financial-related discrimination

Financial-related discrimination statistically significantly accounted for 3.28% and 4.40% of the total effect of wealth on depressive symptoms at 2-year and 4-year follow-ups, respectively (Table 5). Financial-related discrimination statistically significantly explained 4.17% of the total effect of wealth on decreased enjoyment of life at 2-year follow-up, but did not statistically significantly explain the aforementioned total effect at 4-year follow-up. Financial-related discrimination mediated the association between wealth and loneliness at 4-year follow-up by 7.94%. These results may support the hypothesis that financial-related discrimination may explain the association between wealth and psychological well-being related measures.

Table 5 Mediation by financial-related discrimination on the longitudinal associations between wealth (poorest vs. richest) and psychological well-being related measures (n = 8,988)

Findings from additional/sensitivity analyses

Findings from additional analyses controlling for pre-baseline psychological well-being (Wave 4) were consistent for the associations between wealth and follow-up psychological well-being related measures (Table S4). Financial-related discrimination was associated with all psychological well-being related measures at 2-year follow-up, and with depressive symptoms, eudemonic well-being, and loneliness at 4-year follow-up, after controlling pre-baseline psychological well-being (Table S5). Following pre-conditions for mediation, we found consistent evidence that financial-related discrimination mediated the associations between wealth and depressive symptoms at both follow-ups by 3–4% and between wealth and loneliness at 4-year follow-up by 6.85% (Table S6). We found no statistically significant mediation by financial-related discrimination for enjoyment of life and eudemonic well-being. However, mediation by financial-related discrimination was observed for life satisfaction at 2-year follow-up by 3.60%.

Discussion

The current study examined whether experiencing financial-related discrimination is associated with worsening of psychological well-being related measures, and if it in part explains prospective development of wealth-related socioeconomic based inequalities in psychological well-being related measures amongst older adults. Older adults from the poorest (vs. richest) groups were more likely to report experiencing financial-related discrimination. Financial-related discrimination was prospectively associated with increased depressive symptoms and decreased enjoyment of life at 2-year and 4-year follow-ups, and loneliness at 4-year follow-up, with financial-related discrimination explaining 3–8% of the associations between wealth (poorest vs. richest) and psychological well-being related measures. These findings were largely consistent when we controlled for pre-baseline psychological well-being at Wave 4 in the analyses (as opposed to baseline psychological well-being at Wave 5).

Our findings on the longitudinal associations between wealth and psychological well-being are supported by multiple empirical reports demonstrating clear socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being [49, 50]. However, current evidence also indicates that the association between wealth and psychological well-being may not be always positive. A study from the US found that the positive association between income and emotional well-being (i.e., frequency and intensity of emotions which make an individual’s life pleasant or unpleasant) did not progress beyond an income level of $75,000, suggesting that some richer individuals may still experience poor psychological wellbeing [51]. It may be that wealth is attenuated by broader measures of wellbeing amongst older adults such as their levels of resilience, which have been shown to protect older adults against financial difficulties, which may preserve their psychological well-being, despite relatively low wealth [52]. Another study found that extrinsic goal attainment (e.g., pursuit of financial success) positively contributed to despair amongst European older adults, and was unrelated to psychological health, whereas intrinsic goal attainment (e.g., acceptance of death) was associated with higher subjective wellbeing, suggesting the relationship between wealth and wellbeing is more nuanced amongst older populations [53].

To date, the prospective association between financial-related discrimination and psychological well-being related measures has not been examined. The current study furthers knowledge from existing research evidencing the associations between different types of discriminations: weight discrimination [31, 33], sexual-orientation discrimination [54], age discrimination [55] and psychological well-being, by demonstrating how financial-related discrimination may also contribute negatively towards English older adults’ psychological well-being. There are multiple potential mechanisms through which financial-related discrimination may explain longitudinal associations between wealth as a SES measure and psychological well-being. Consistent with IH Meyer [56]’s minority stress framework, which describes relationships between proximal (e.g., perceived discrimination) or distal (e.g., experienced discrimination) stressors and mental distress, perceived financial-related discrimination (as a proximal stressor), may increase the risk of reduced well-being, through stress. This was illustrated by NE Adler and AC Snibbe [57], who produced a model depicting pathways between SES and health, whereby declining SES exposes an individual to increased stress (e.g., financial-related discrimination) and decreased resources to deal with stress (e.g., mental health service availability), resulting in larger psychological responses and increased vulnerability to disease [58]. ML Hatzenbuehler [59] extended the minority stress framework by proposing cognitive (e.g., rumination), affective (e.g., emotion dysregulation) and social (e.g., lack of support) psychological mediators through which proximal or distal minority stressors become associated with psychopathology, which may provide causal mechanisms underlying associations between financial-related discrimination and decreased psychological well-being identified within the current study. This implication is supported by a cross-sectional analysis of US low-income women which revealed internalised poverty stigma and depression were partially mediated by psychological factors like self-esteem and fear of rejection [18].

However, financial-related discrimination only explained a small proportion of prospective associations between wealth and psychological well-being. Significant associations between wealth and psychological well-being, as well as between financial-related discrimination and psychological well-being (independent of wealth), suggest that unmeasured mediators may contribute to this association. For example, a previous study using data of older adults across European countries found that subjective social status (3%) explained the association between SES and life satisfaction [60]. Social support, stress and health-related behaviors may also explain the association between SES and psychological well-being [8, 10]. Furthermore, a relatively low prevalence of financial-related discrimination (6%) in the sample may limit the variability to detect significant findings. In addition, compared to overt racism or sexism, financial-related discrimination may be more difficult to accurately identify or perceive as being caused by financial circumstance, which may make estimating financial-related discrimination prevalence challenging [61]. This may explain why financial-related discrimination was a small yet significant mediator of associations between SES and psychological well-being in the current study.

The relatively small amount of variance that experiencing financial-related discrimination explained for the relationship between wealth and worsening of psychological well-being measures may also be due to the older age of participants in this study. Financial related discrimination was measured at a single time point in old age, and by old age, significant wealth-based inequalities in psychological well-being have already developed. Financial related discrimination experienced earlier in life may therefore play a larger role in the development of socio-economic inequalities in psychological well-being. Although more common among participants in the poorest wealth category (9%), participants in the richest wealth category also reported financial-related discrimination (4%). This suggests that experiencing financial-related discrimination is not limited to very low-income families and may be contributing negatively to the psychological well-being of individuals across socioeconomic statuses.

The use of the health stigma and discrimination framework, designed to inform research and interventions tackling intersecting forms of stigma (e.g., race, gender, class), offers both upstream (e.g., anti-stigma education) and downstream (e.g., targeted psychological therapy) intervention targets [62]. Based on the present results, policy action should address both determinants of financial-related discrimination (e.g., stereotyping, prejudice), whilst supporting individuals most at risk of financial-related discrimination (e.g., low SES) identified within the current study. This may be the most efficacious approach in tackling wealth-related socioeconomic based inequalities in older adults’ psychological well-being.

Strengths, limitations, and directions for future research

This study is the first to evidence that financial-related discrimination predicts worsening psychological well-being, as well as partially explaining prospective associations between wealth-related SES and psychological well-being related measures. Strengths include use of a longitudinal design, across a nationally representative sample of older adults, which increases the findings generalisability. Additionally, use of MICE to account for missing data across several variables may reduce selection bias. Findings were also largely consistent when either pre-baseline or baseline psychological well-being was controlled for in the analyses.

The study has limitations, including the predominately White, ethnically homogenous sample of English older adults obtained from ELSA, which prevent generalisation of the psychological implications of financial-related discrimination within ethnically diverse populations, or adults aged 18–49. Therefore, future research should measure financial-related discrimination prevalence across populations aged 18–49. Despite that wealth is a likely reliable SES measure given its objectivity, individuals may have had unequal access to household resources, and this would result in an incomplete picture of available financial resources, or the cumulative effect of deprivation, or privilege, prior to retirement [63, 64]. However, previous research found wealth was the most robust SES measure amongst ELSA datasets, compared to any other SES indicator (e.g., education, employment) [22, 24]. The association between wealth and psychological well-being in the current study was less vulnerable to reverse causation, given analyses were controlled for baseline or pre-baseline psychological well-being. However, as wealth and financial-related discrimination were assessed in the same wave, and both measures were self-reported by the participants, this may introduce same-source bias which may influence the findings to some extent.

The current study provides no information on the source of perceived financial-related discrimination and is based on self-report, which is prone to bias. Further research will benefit from better understanding factors contributing to financial related discrimination. SES related variables may interact to increase the risk of experiencing this form of discrimination. H Tajfel and JC Turner [65] suggest that discrimination is more likely to be experienced if an individual is perceived as a member of an outgroup, which may suggest that individuals with a personal SES incongruent to their neighbourhood-level SES may also be more likely to experience financial-related discrimination. Therefore, future research should determine whether neighbourhood-level SES (e.g., index of multiple deprivation) influences individuals’ risk of perceived financial-related discrimination, enabling targeted mental health service provision for individuals most at risk of perceived financial-related discrimination, and subsequently poorer psychological well-being.

Due to information on the experience of discrimination being limited to Wave 5, we were unable to conduct further analyses exploring whether changes in discrimination experiences may moderate the association between wealth and psychological well-being, or the changes in both. Future research may benefit from investigating whether psychological well-being may improve or decline in response to changes in wealth, and whether the related improvement or decline in psychological well-being is more pronounced in those who start experiencing financial-related discrimination, or cease to experience it. When data permit, adopting structural equation models, as demonstrated in [47], can help to examine how changes in psychological well-being, following changes in wealth, may vary based on changes in financial-related discrimination.

Conclusions

Financial-related discrimination may increase risk for worse psychological well—being and explain a small proportion of socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being.

Data availability

Data are available at https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.5255/UKDA-Series-200011 with the permission of UK Data Services. The codes used to generate the findings presented in this study are available at https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.17605/OSF.IO/2A4MD.

References

  1. GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the global burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry. 2022;9(2):137–50.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Arias D, Saxena S, Verguet S. Quantifying the global burden of mental disorders and their economic value. eClinicalMedicine. 2022;54:101675.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Zhang A, Gagné T, Walsh D, Ciancio A, Proto E, McCartney G. Trends in psychological distress in Great Britain, 1991–2019: evidence from three representative surveys. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2023;77(7):468–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, Ansseau M. Socioeconomic inequalities in Depression: a Meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(2):98–112.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Steptoe A. P Zaninotto 2020 Lower socioeconomic status and the acceleration of aging: an outcome-wide analysis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 117 26 14911–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Bai X, Li Z, Chen J, Liu C, Wu X. Socioeconomic inequalities in mental distress and life satisfaction among older Chinese men and women: the role of family functioning. Health Soc Care Commun. 2020;28(4):1270–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Tapia-Muñoz T, Staudinger UM, Allel K, Steptoe A, Miranda-Castillo C, Medina JT, Calvo E. Income inequality and its relationship with loneliness prevalence: a cross-sectional study among older adults in the US and 16 European countries. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(12):e0274518.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Stansfeld SA, Head J, Fuhrer R, Wardle J, Cattell V. Social inequalities in depressive symptoms and physical functioning in the Whitehall II study: exploring a common cause explanation. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2003;57(5):361–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Zhang Y, Su D, Chen Y, Tan M, Chen X. Effect of socioeconomic status on the physical and mental health of the elderly: the mediating effect of social participation. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Matthews KA, Gallo LC. Psychological perspectives on pathways linking Socioeconomic Status and Physical Health. Ann Rev Psychol. 2010;62(1):501–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Oishi S, Kesebir S, Diener E. Income inequality and happiness. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(9):1095–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Cheung F, Lucas RE. Income inequality is associated with stronger social comparison effects: the effect of relative income on life satisfaction. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2016;110(2):332–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Goffman E. Stigma: notes on the management of Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall; 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Link BG, Phelan JC. Conceptualizing Stigma. Ann Rev Sociol. 2001;27(1):363–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Hatzenbuehler ML, Phelan JC, Link BG. Stigma as a Fundamental cause of Population Health inequalities. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(5):813–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Jokela M, Fuller-Rowell TE. Changing associations between socioeconomic status and self-reported discrimination from the 1990s to the 2010s in the United States. Int J Psychol. 2022;57(6):760–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Fuller-Rowell TE, Curtis DS, Chae DH, Ryff CD. Longitudinal health consequences of socioeconomic disadvantage: examining perceived discrimination as a mediator. Health Psychol. 2018;37(5):491–500.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Mickelson KD, Williams SL. Perceived Stigma of Poverty and Depression: examination of interpersonal and Intrapersonal mediators. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2008;27(9):903–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Amirova A, Rimes KA, Hackett RA. Perceived discrimination in middle-aged and older adults: comparison between England and the United States. Front Public Health 2022, 10.

  20. Rogers NT, Banks J, Nazroo J, Steptoe A. English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA). In: Encyclopedia of Geropsychology edn. Edited by Pachana NA. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2017: 797–806.

  21. Banks J, Batty GD, Breedvelt J, Coughlin K, Crawford R, Marmot M, Nazroo J, Oldfield Z, Steel N, Steptoe A et al. English Longitudinal Study of Ageing: Waves 0–9, 1998–2019. [data collection]. In., 38th Edition edn: UK Data Service. SN; 2023.

  22. Torres JL, Lima-Costa MF, Marmot M, de Oliveira C. Wealth and disability in later life: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). PLoS ONE. 2016;11(11):e0166825.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. McMunn A, Nazroo J, Breeze E. Inequalities in health at older ages: a longitudinal investigation of the onset of illness and survival effects in England. Age Ageing. 2009;38(2):181–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Demakakos P, Biddulph JP, Bobak M, Marmot MG. Wealth and mortality at older ages: a prospective cohort study. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2016;70(4):346–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Makaroun LK, Brown RT, Diaz-Ramirez LG, Ahalt C, Boscardin WJ, Lang-Brown S, Lee S. Wealth-Associated disparities in Death and disability in the United States and England. JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(12):1745–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: Socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. 1997;2(3):335–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Robinson E, Sutin A, Daly M. Perceived weight discrimination mediates the prospective relation between obesity and depressive symptoms in U.S. and U.K. adults. Health Psychol. 2017;36(2):112–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Turvey CL, Wallace RB, Herzog R. A revised CES-D measure of depressive symptoms and a DSM-based measure of major depressive episodes in the elderly. Int Psychogeriatr. 1999;11(2):139–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Putra IGNE, Daly M, Sutin A, Steptoe A, Robinson E. The psychological legacy of past obesity and early mortality: evidence from two longitudinal studies. BMC Med. 2023;21(1):448.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Hyde M, Wiggins RD, Higgs P, Blane DB. A measure of quality of life in early old age: the theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19). Aging Ment Health. 2003;7(3):186–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Putra I, Daly M, Sutin A, Steptoe A, Robinson E. Psychological pathways explaining the prospective association between obesity and physiological dysregulation. Health Psychol. 2023;42(7):472–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with Life Scale. J Pers Assess. 1985;49(1):71–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Jackson SE, Beeken RJ, Wardle J. Obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and psychological well-being in older adults in England. Obesity. 2015;23(5):1105–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two Population-Based studies. Res Aging. 2004;26(6):655–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Davies K, Maharani A, Chandola T, Todd C, Pendleton N. The longitudinal relationship between loneliness, social isolation, and frailty in older adults in England: a prospective analysis. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2021;2(2):e70–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Godhwani S, Jivraj S, Marshall A, Bécares L. Comparing subjective and objective neighbourhood deprivation and their association with health over time among older adults in England. Health Place. 2019;55:51–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Shields MA, Price SW. Exploring the Economic and Social determinants of Psychological Well-Being and Perceived Social Support in England. J Royal Stat Soc Ser A: Stat Soc. 2005;168(3):513–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1986;51(6):1173–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Kohler U, Karlson KB, Holm A. Comparing coefficients of nested nonlinear probability models. Stata J. 2011;11(3):420–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Holden KC, Smeeding TM. The poor, the Rich, and the Insecure Elderly Caught in between. Milbank Q. 1990;68(2):191–219.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Deckers K, Cadar D, van Boxtel MPJ, Verhey FRJ, Steptoe A, Köhler S. Modifiable risk factors explain socioeconomic inequalities in Dementia Risk: evidence from a Population-based prospective cohort study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;71:549–57.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Putra IGNE, McInerney AM, Robinson E, Deschênes SS. Neighbourhood characteristics and socioeconomic inequalities in child mental health: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses in the growing up in Ireland Study. In.; 2023.

  43. Azur MJ, Stuart EA, Frangakis C, Leaf PJ. Multiple imputation by chained equations: what is it and how does it work? Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2011;20(1):40–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. 2011;30(4):377–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Camacho C, Straatmann VS, Day JC, Taylor-Robinson D. Development of a predictive risk model for school readiness at age 3 years using the UK Millennium Cohort Study. BMJ Open. 2019;9(6):e024851.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Berglund P. Multiple imputation using chained equations: a comparison of Stata. SAS, IVEware and R. In.; 2014.

  47. Hoffmann R, Kröger H, Pakpahan E. The reciprocal relationship between material factors and health in the life course: evidence from SHARE and ELSA. Eur J Ageing. 2018;15(4):379–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Aittomäki A, Martikainen P, Laaksonen M, Lahelma E, Rahkonen O. Household economic resources, labour-market advantage and health problems - a study on causal relationships using prospective register data. Soc Sci Med. 2012;75(7):1303–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Diener E, Biswas-Diener R. Will money increase subjective Well-Being? Soc Indic Res. 2002;57(2):119–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Navarro-Carrillo G, Alonso-Ferres M, Moya M, Valor-Segura I. Socioeconomic status and Psychological Well-Being: revisiting the role of subjective socioeconomic status. Front Psychol 2020, 11.

  51. Kahneman D. A Deaton 2010 High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107 38 16489–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Kashapova E. Analysis of the Relationship Between Resilience and Financial Well-Being of Older People. In: Proceeding of the International Science and Technology Conference FarEastСon 2021: 2022// 2022; Singapore: Springer Nature Singapore; 2022: 535–541.

  53. Van Hiel A, Vansteenkiste M. Ambitions fulfilled? The effects of intrinsic and extrinsic goal attainment on older adults’ Ego-Integrity and Death attitudes. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2009;68(1):27–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Jackson SE, Hackett RA, Grabovac I, Smith L, Steptoe A. Perceived discrimination, health and wellbeing among middle-aged and older lesbian, gay and bisexual people: a prospective study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(5):e0216497.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Jackson SE, Hackett RA, Steptoe A. Associations between age discrimination and health and wellbeing: cross-sectional and prospective analysis of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Lancet Public Health. 2019;4(4):e200–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Meyer IH. Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(5):674–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Adler NE, Snibbe AC. The role of psychosocial processes in explaining the gradient between socioeconomic status and health. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2003;12(4):119–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Gallo LC, Matthews KA. Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: do negative emotions play a role? Psychol Bull. 2003;129(1):10–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hatzenbuehler ML. How does sexual minority stigma get under the skin? A psychological mediation framework. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(5):707–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Moreno-Agostino D, de la Fuente J, Leonardi M, Koskinen S, Tobiasz-Adamczyk B, Sánchez-Niubò A, Chatterji S, Haro JM, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Miret M. Mediators of the socioeconomic status and life satisfaction relationship in older adults: a multi-country structural equation modeling approach. Aging Ment Health. 2021;25(3):585–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Link BG, Phelan JC. Labeling and Stigma. In: Handbook of the Sociology of Mental Health edn. Edited by Aneshensel CS, Phelan JC, Bierman A. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2013: 525–541.

  62. Stangl AL, Earnshaw VA, Logie CH, van Brakel W, Simbayi C, Barré L, Dovidio I. The health stigma and discrimination Framework: a global, crosscutting framework to inform research, intervention development, and policy on health-related stigmas. BMC Med. 2019;17(1):31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Duncan GJ, Daly MC, McDonough P, Williams DR. Optimal Indicators of Socioeconomic Status for Health Research. Am J Public Health. 2002;92(7):1151–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Robert S, House JS. SES differentials in Health by Age and Alternative indicators of SES. J Aging Health. 1996;8(3):359–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Tajfel H, Turner JC. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Intergroup relations: Essential readings edn. Edited by Hogg MA, Abrams D: Psychology Press; 2001: 94–109.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is funded by the National Institute on Aging (R01AG017644) and by a consortium of UK Government Departments coordinated by the National Institute of Health and Care Research (198/1047).

Funding

This work received funding from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC), a part of the United Kingdom Research and Innovation (UKRI) (ES/V017594/1). The views stated in this work are of the authors only.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

L.B. and I.G.N.E.P. conceptualised the study, conducted data analysis, and interpreted the results. L.B. wrote the initial manuscript. E.R. and I.G.N.E.P. critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to I Gusti Ngurah Edi Putra.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We used data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). ELSA has received ethical approval from different institutional review boards for its waves (https://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/ethical-approval). Informed consent was sought from all the ELSA participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bridson, L., Robinson, E. & Putra, I. Financial-related discrimination and socioeconomic inequalities in psychological well-being related measures: a longitudinal study. BMC Public Health 24, 1008 (2024). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-024-18417-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-024-18417-w

Keywords