Skip to main content

Mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language development among toddlers: results from a large-scale survey

Abstract

Background

Despite young children’s widespread use of mobile devices, little research exists on this use and its association with children’s language development. The aim of this study was to examine the associations between mobile device screen time and language comprehension and expressive language skills. An additional aim was to examine whether three factors related to the domestic learning environment modify the associations.

Methods

The study uses data from the Danish large-scale survey TRACES among two- and three-year-old children (n = 31,125). Mobile device screen time was measured as time spent on mobile devices on a normal day. Measurement of language comprehension and expressive language skills was based on subscales from the Five to Fifteen Toddlers questionnaire. Multivariable linear regression was used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language development and logistic regression to examine the risk of experiencing significant language difficulties. Joint exposure analyses were used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language development difficulties in combination with three other factors related to the domestic learning environment: parental education, reading to the child and child TV/PC screen time.

Results

High mobile device screen time of one hour or more per day was significantly associated with poorer language development scores and higher odds for both language comprehension difficulties (1–2 h: AOR = 1.30; ≥ 2 h: AOR = 1.42) and expressive language skills difficulties (1–2 h: AOR = 1.19; ≥ 2 h: AOR = 1.46). The results suggest that reading frequently to the child partly buffers the negative effect of high mobile device screen time on language comprehension difficulties but not on expressive language skills difficulties. No modifying effect of parental education and time spent by the child on TV/PC was found.

Conclusions

Mobile device screen time of one hour or more per day is associated with poorer language development among toddlers. Reading frequently to the child may have a buffering effect on language comprehension difficulties but not on expressive language skills difficulties.

Peer Review reports

Children’s language skills help them to develop socially and academically, which implies that the acquisition of language is one of the most important processes in child development. Conversely, language delays and disorders in childhood have been associated with problem behaviors [1], and have also been associated with poorer academic achievement when these children start at school and later in their lives [2,3,4]. From birth, language develops as the child communicates with their parents and other significant persons [5]. This language development can be enhanced through a cognitively stimulating learning environment such as reading to the child [6, 7].

During the last decade, mobile devices such as tablets and smartphones have become an integrated part of daily life in families. Already from infancy, children are exposed to mobile devices [8,9,10]. Mobile devices provide children and parents with easy access to entertainment, not only in the home setting but also outside the home. Although this easy access to screen-based entertainment is convenient for parents in some situations [11], it may also pose a risk to toddlers’ language development [12, 13]. Despite the growing exposure of toddlers to mobile devices, little research has focused on mobile device screen time and language development among toddlers [13].

To date, most research on the association between a child’s screen time and language development has focused on either TV or a combination of TV and other digital devices such as smartphones, tablets and computers [13]. These studies have found that children interact less with their parents while the children are watching TV [14, 15], that very young children require adult interaction to learn from screen-based media [7, 16], and that parents are less sensitive to child cues when the parents are engaged in screen use themselves [17]. A study among pre-school children found a negative association between the children’s screen-based media use and white matter microstructural integrity in core areas of the brain supporting language [18]. Although cross-sectional in its design, the findings of this study are in accordance with the results of studies on the association between screen use and language development [13, 19]. In a systematic review and meta-analysis of 38 studies focusing on screen time on various types of devices and language skills among 0-12-year-olds, Madigan et al. (2020) found a negative association between screen time and child language skills. Additionally, they found that the potential benefits of screen-based media were less likely among younger children, although co-viewing and educational content was associated with a small positive effect on child language skills. However, out of the 38 studies included in the review, only two studies focused specifically on mobile device screen time among toddlers [9, 20]. Contrary to Madigan et al. [13], Jing et al. [21] found an overall positive effect of screen media exposure on vocabulary among 0-6-year-old children in a recent meta-analysis of 63 studies. However, in moderator analyses, only experimental studies (testing the effect of exposure to media with an educational content) showed a positive significant effect on vocabulary, with stronger effect found for e-books than apps or games. The meta-analysis comprising studies conducted in a naturalistic setting found no overall effect on vocabulary, with the exception of exposure to media with an educational content. In line with Madigan et al. [13], the meta-analysis by Jing et al. [21] also found larger effects in samples with an average age of 36 months or older.

The effect of screen-based media on language development most likely depends on the content and the context the media are used in [13, 21, 22]. Thus, there are reasons to believe that mobile device screen time may affect language development differently than other screen-based media such as TV and computers. Firstly, mobile devices differ from TV by being portable and therefore easily accessible in various contexts. Secondly, they are handheld, thus co-viewing may be less pronounced [23, 24]. Finally, mobile devices differ from TV by being interactive and by providing reward stimuli [7]. These characteristics may affect child language development either directly, through the content and format which may enhance or inhibit language development, or indirectly, by decreasing the opportunities for children to engage in communicating situations in everyday life [7, 23]. To our knowledge, only few studies have examined the association between mobile device screen time and language development. Of these, two studies suggest a negative association between time spent on mobile devices and expressive language among toddlers [9, 20], while no significant association was found with other communication delays [9] or language comprehension [20]. Another recent study found no significant association between prolonged use of touch screens and overall communication and language development delay among toddlers [25]. The association between mobile device screen time and language development among toddlers is thus unclear.

With child development being multifactorial, the association between mobile device screen time and the child’s language development may also be affected by other activities a child engages in during the day. Research has found that both parental and child screen time is associated with less optimal parent-child communication [7, 14, 15, 17]. However, the parent-child interaction, including language-stimulating activities and book reading, has been found to enhance language development [7, 26, 27]). Thus, Sundqvist et al. (2021) found interactional turn-taking to be a stronger predictor of vocabulary among 25 month old children than TV content and parental device use during daily child routines. Also, the association between mobile device screen time and language development may vary depending on whether the mobile device is the primary unit for screen-based activities or whether the child spends additional time on watching TV thus increasing the overall screen time and risk of missing out on parent-child communication and language stimulating activities [7, 14, 15]. Finally, the overall learning environment in terms of language stimulating activities, norms and attitudes towards screen use in the family may also play a role. These are factors that are associated with parental resources, including parental education [4, 28,29,30] and may be part of protective or harmful mechanisms [31]. To date, there is, however, a lack of studies that examine the association between mobile device screen time and language development in combination with other factors related to the home and learning environment of the family.

Using a large cross-sectional survey among 31,125 two- and three-year-old children, the objectives of the present study were (1) to examine the association between mobile device screen time and language development among toddlers, and further, (2) to examine this association in combination with three other factors related to the domestic learning environment: parental education, frequency of reading to the child, and child TV/PC screen time. Based on previous research, our hypothesis was that higher mobile device screen time would be associated with poorer language outcomes among toddlers. Additionally, that factors related to domestic learning environment would modify the associations, with a favorable environment reducing the potential negative associations between high mobile device screen time and language development.

Compared to previous studies which have primarily been based on small samples [20, 25], the current study is the largest study to date focusing on mobile device screen time and language development among toddlers. To our knowledge it is also the first to explore this association in combination with other factors related to the domestic learning environment.

Method

Study design and population

This study uses cross-sectional baseline data from the Danish longitudinal web-based survey ‘TRACES – Children’s development and well-being throughout life’ [32]. The overall purpose of TRACES is to increase knowledge about small children’s development and well-being and to examine how circumstances and interventions early in life affect well-being and welfare. The baseline survey of TRACES was conducted for VIVE between October, 2017 and September, 2018 by Statistics Denmark and comprises data from 52,010 children aged nine to ten months, two years and three years, equally distributed across the three age groups. Statistics Denmark sampled 90,387 children and subsequently invited the parents of the children to participate in the questionnaire survey through a secure electronic mailbox (e-Boks) the month after their child turned nine months, two years or three years. The questionnaire [33] took approximately half an hour to complete. To accommodate a lower response rate among children from disadvantaged families, children from potentially disadvantaged families were oversampled in the ratio 60/40 in each age group. Potentially disadvantaged families were defined as families where mothers met one or more of the following criteria at child birth: young age, no education other than compulsory school, not cohabiting with the other parent or having received welfare benefits one, two or three years before the birth of the child. Children from the potentially disadvantaged families group were sampled first, and subsequently, a random sample of the remaining children in each age group was sampled. The overall response rate was 57.5% of the sampled children (children from potentially disadvantaged families: 48.4%; other children: 71.1%).

The present study only uses data from the two- and three-year-old children. The study sample was furthermore restricted to the 31,125 children with available data on both language development and mobile device screen time (90.6%). This sample is close to representative of the full population of children in Denmark in these two age groups, in that the sample displays only minor differences in child age, parental education, employment status and parental migration status when compared with the full population of children in Denmark in these two age groups (see supplementary table S1).

Measures

Child mobile device screen time

Child mobile device screen time was measured by the item “On a normal weekday, how much time does [name of the child] spend on an iPad, tablet or smartphone (e.g. playing games or watching a movie)?” The response categories were: “No time at all”, “less than half an hour a day”, “half to 1 hour a day, “1–2 hours a day” and “More than 2 hours a day”. In order to provide hourly intervals, “less than half an hour” and “half to 1 hour” were collapsed, and sensitivity analyses showed no substantial differences in results when this was done.

Language development

Measurement of language comprehension and expressive language skills was based on two subscales from the parent-reported Five to Fifteen Toddlers questionnaire (FTF-Toddlers) [34]. This questionnaire is a toddler version (two to five years) of the Five to Fifteen questionnaire (FTF), which is a Nordic questionnaire for evaluation of development and behavior, including language development, among five- to fifteen-year-olds [35]. While the FTF has been validated in various settings [36], the FTF-Toddlers is still being tested [37]. The items of the two subscales are statements about the child’s language development (see supplementary tables S2 and S3 for items). The parent is asked to report how they feel that their child functions compared to children of the same age on a three-point Likert-type scale: “Does not apply” (0), “Applies sometimes/to some extent” [1] and “Applies” [2]. Sum scores are calculated for each subscale, with higher scores indicating poorer language development.

Language comprehension was measured by the seven-item FTF-Toddlers language comprehension scale (e.g. “Has difficulty understanding words” and “Has difficulty understanding simple instructions”) with a score range of 0–14 and a Cronbach’s α of 0.83. While the FTF language comprehension scale was unidimensional, initial exploratory factor analysis (iterated principal factor analysis with oblique (promax) rotation) pointed towards a two-dimensional structure of the ten-item FTF-Toddlers spoken language subscale reflecting: (1) expressive language skills (items 61–65) and (2) more speech-related aspects such as stuttering and voice usage (items 66–70) (see supplementary tables S3 and S4). Based on conceptual considerations and unidimensionality being a fundamental requirement of psychometric scales we omitted items on speech related aspects in the present study. Instead we focused on expressive language skills measured by a five-item scale comprising items such as “Has difficulties saying single words and short sentences” and “Has difficulty speaking so that his/her parents understand him/her” (score range: 0–10, Cronbach’s α: 0.86).

Besides the two scales on language development, two dichotomous variables of difficulties with language comprehension and expressive language skills were used in the logistic regression analyses. Currently, no norms for the FTF-Toddlers exist. However, the sample used in the present study is close to representative for the Danish population of 2- and 3-year olds (supplementary table S1). A recent systematic review have found a prevalence of low language skills to vary between 3.4% and 17.5% (M = 10.7%) in children aged between 1 and 4 years [38]. Therefore, scores at or above the 90th percentile in the child’s gender and age group were used as cut-offs for being at risk of language difficulties (hereafter: difficulties) (see supplementary table S5). This is in agreement with the procedure used in studies using the original FTF questionnaire where a score above the 90th percentile marks the threshold for clinical concern [39].

Covariates

Child gender, child age, parental education, parental employment status, and parental migration status were based on administrative data from Statistics Denmark. Parental education was measured as the highest achieved education of the parents and categorized into short (short-cycle higher education, high school or less), medium (medium-cycle higher education) and long education (long-cycle higher education). Parental employment status was measured as the number of parents in employment. Because of known differences in child-rearing practices among different cultures, also when it comes to screen time use, parental migration status was included in the analyses [6]. Parental migration status was classified according to the standards of Statistics Denmark [40] and categorized into: (1) both parents of Danish origin, (2) one parent of Danish origin and one either immigrant or descendant of immigrants, and (3) both parents being immigrants or descendants of immigrants.

Parental mental well-being, parental preoccupation with smartphone, reading stories to the child and child screen time on TV or PC were assessed in the survey. Parental mental well-being was measured by WHO-5 [41], with a score below 50 indicating risk of depression or stress. Parental preoccupation with smartphone as a proxy for smartphone use was measured by a 0–2 point index based on two dichotomized indicators of being highly preoccupied with one’s smartphone: “Thinking of the mobile phone when not using it” and “frequently checking the mobile phone so as not to miss out on what is going on” (indication of high preoccupation for both statements was signaled by the answers “highly true” or “completely true” (coded 1), while no indication of high preoccupation was signaled by the answers “Not at all true”, “a little bit true”, “somewhat true” or “true to a large extent” (coded 0)) ). Child screen time on TV/PC was measured by the item “On a normal weekday, how much time does [name of the child] spend watching TV or movies on the TV or on a computer?” with response categories coded similarly to mobile device screen time. Finally, Reading stories to the child in the past month [42] was dichotomized in order to create an indicator for frequent (on “a daily” or “almost daily” basis) and infrequent reading (“a few times a week”, “a few times a month” “rarely” or “not at all”), respectively.

Statistical analyses

Multivariable linear regression with mobile device screen time treated as a categorical variable was used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language comprehension and expressive language skills score. In order to examine how mobile device screen time was associated with indication of risk of language difficulties relative to the majority of children in the same age and gender group we used logistic regression. Logistic regression was used to estimate odds ratios (ORs) for language development scores at or above the 90% percentile in the child’s age and gender group. The analyses were conducted in three steps: a crude model (Model 1), a model adjusted for sociodemographic variables (Model 2), and a full model where factors related to the child’s home environment (parental mental well-being, parental preoccupation with smartphone, screen time on TV/PC and reading to the child) were added (Model 3). This approach was chosen in order to disentangle the effect of adjusting for sociodemographic variables and additionally the home environment. All included confounders were significantly associated with both mobile device screen time (chi2-test, p < 0.001), and language comprehension and expressive language skills (Mood’s median test, p < 0.001).

In accordance with STROBE recommendations, the potential modifying effect of three dichotomized factors related to the learning environment of the home (parental education, frequency of reading to the child and child TV/PC screen time, respectively) was determined using joint exposure analyses [43]. In this way not only the separate effects of high mobile device screen time and the dichotomized home environment factor on language development but also the joint effect of the two variables can be examined. Thus, it is possible to identify protective or harmful combinations of screen time and the included factors related to the learning environment. In the joint exposure analyses, high mobile device screen time was defined as ≥ 1 h based on the guidelines by WHO [44] and the American Academy of Pediatrics [45]. Thus, children with low mobile device screen time and no exposure in TV/PC screen time, frequency of being read for or whose parents’ highest education was categorized as short, respectively were considered the reference group in the three joint exposure analyses. Taking a public health perspective [43], test for effect modification was defined as departure from additivity and was conducted using Rothman’s method for creating interaction terms [46]. Synergy indexes (SI) adjusted for confounding were calculated by the equation (OREXP++ - 1)/([OREXP+− -1] + [OREXP−+ -1]) with 95% confidence intervals calculated according to the methods described by Hosmer and Lemeshow [47]. A SI can go from 0 to infinity, where a SI of 1 (exactly additivity) means no effect modification i.e. the combined effect of the two exposures being equal to the sum of the absolute effects of the two exposures. A SI above 1 means more than additivity of the effects of the two exposures, i.e. the combined effect being larger than the sum of the absolute effects of the two exposures. A SI below 1 means less than additivity of the two effects. All analyses were conducted using the statistical software SAS version 9.4.

Missing data

Out of the full study sample of 31,125 children, a total of 28,141 children (90.4%) had complete data on all covariates included in the fully adjusted model 3. Only few significant differences were found between the full study sample and the subsample of children with complete data. Children with complete data were significantly less likely to come from a family with short educations (41% vs. 42% in the full study sample) and families where both parents were immigrants or descendants of immigrants (13.5% vs. 14.1%) (See supplementary table S1). The analyses were based on the full study sample. Complete case analyses (n = 28,141) of model 1 and model 2 revealed comparable results.

Results

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. The sample was evenly divided in children aged 2- and 3 years. Likewise, around half of the sample were girls (48.9%). A total of 42.0% had parents with a short education and 6.4% of the children came from families with two unemployed parents. The majority of children (74.7%) had two parents with Danish origin while 14.1% had two parents who were immigrants or descendants of immigrants. Overall, 72.2% of the children spent time on mobile devices during a normal weekday (i.e. the five weekdays Monday to Friday). Most children (62.2%) spent less than one hour on mobile devices, while 10% spent one hour or more. In comparison, 88% of the children spent time on the TV/PC, with 75.5% using the TV/PC for less than one hour. The distributions of language comprehension and expressive language skills in the sample were positively skewed for both 2- and 3- year old children (see Table 2).

Table 1 Sample characteristics: (1) frequency of exposure, and covariates in the sample and (2) prevalence of mobile device screen use ≥ 1 h by covariates

Table 1 also shows the prevalence of ≥ 1 h mobile device screen time by covariates. Mobile device screen time was significantly associated with all sociodemographic characteristics, with screen time of one hour or more being more prevalent among children from families with short educations, among children from families with a lower attachment to the labor market and among children from families where both parents were immigrants or descendants of immigrants. Likewise, all factors related to the home environment were significantly associated with mobile device screen use. Thus, mobile device screen time of one hour or more was more prevalent among children with higher screen time on TV/PC, among children being read to a few times a week or less frequently, among children having a parent with low mental well-being and among children of parents with indicators of high preoccupation with smartphone.

The distribution of language comprehension skills and expressive language skills differed significantly between children with mobile device screen time of one hour or more and children with less screen time (Mood’s median test, p < 0.001) with higher median scores among children with mobile device screen time of one hour or more (Table 2).

Table 2 Distribution of the language domains presented as (1) median scores and interquartile ranges (IQR) among 2- and 3-year old boys and girls and (2) and frequency of children of risk of difficulties

Mobile device screen time and language development

Table 3  presents the results of the linear regression analyses with positive B-coefficients indicating poorer language development. For both language comprehension and expressive language skills, spending one hour or more on mobile devices on normal weekdays was associated with poorer language development. Compared to children with no mobile device screen time, the crude B-coefficients (Model 1) for children spending more than two hours on mobile devices were 1.36 (95% CI: 1.13, 1.60) for language comprehension and 0.99 (95% CI: 0.76, 1.23) for expressive language skills. After first adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics (Model 2) and then further adjusting for home environment characteristics (Model 3), B-coefficients decreased markedly for children spending more than two hours on mobile devices, but remained significant. Thus, the average language comprehension score of children who spent more than two hours on mobile devices was 0.48 higher (95% CI: 0.23, 0.73) (on a scale ranging from 0 to 14) compared to the score of children with no mobile device screen time, indicating poorer language development among children with higher mobile device screen time. For expressive language skills, the corresponding B-coefficient was 0.46 (95% CI: 0.21, 0.71) on a scale ranging from 0 to 10. Spending less than one hour on mobile devices was associated with a slightly better language comprehension and expressive language skills score, but the estimates diminished with adjustment for sociodemographic characteristics and was no longer significant for expressive language skills.

Table 3 Crude and adjusted regression coefficients for language comprehension and expressive language skills development

Mobile device screen time association with language comprehension difficulties and expressive language skills difficulties

Table 4 presents the crude and adjusted odds ratios for having difficulties with language comprehension and expressive language. For both outcomes, a significant association between mobile device screen time of one hour or more and being at risk of having language development difficulties was found. Children spending more than two hours on mobile devices during a normal weekday had an OR of 3.68 (95% CI: 2.94, 4.60) for language comprehension difficulties and an OR of 3.12 (95% CI: 2.48, 3.92) for difficulties with expressive language skills compared to children with no screen time (Model 1). When adjusted for sociodemographic and home environment characteristics (Model 3), these ORs decreased markedly to 1.42 (95% CI: 1.08, 1.88) for language comprehension and to 1.46 (95% CI: 1.10, 1.93) for expressive language skills difficulties. For the category of less than one hour of mobile device screen time, a slightly lower OR for expressive language skills difficulties was found when compared to children with no screen time.

Table 4 Crude and adjusted odds ratios for language comprehension and expressive language skills difficulties

Joint exposure analyses of mobile device screen time and factors related to the domestic learning environment

Table 5 presents the results of the analyses combining mobile device screen time with parental education, reading to the child, and child TV/PC screen time. Compared to children with low mobile device screen time and whose parents had medium/long educations, children of parents with short education (OR = 1.72; 95% CI: 1.57, 1.88) or exposed to high mobile device screen time (OR = 1.93; 95% CI: 1.64, 2.27) were at higher risk of language comprehension difficulties. The corresponding ORs for expressive language skills difficulties were 1.30 (95% CI: 1.19, 1.41) among children of parents with short parental education and 1.72 (95% CI: 1.47, 2.02) when exposed to high mobile device screen time. For both types of language development difficulties, the ORs increased when exposed to both risk factors. However, the test for effect modification did not support departure from additivity.

Compared to children with low screen time on both TV/PC and mobile devices, children with either high screen time (≥ 1 h) on TV/PC (OR = 1.51; 95% CI: 1.35, 1.68) or high mobile device screen time (OR = 1.91; 95% CI: 1.65, 2.20) were both at higher risk of language comprehension difficulties, with a tendency towards the strongest association between mobile device screen time and difficulties. Likewise, the corresponding ORs for expressive language skills difficulties were 1.38 (95% CI: 1.23, 1.54) when exposed to high screen time on TV/PC and 1.71 (95% CI: 1.48, 1.98) when exposed to high mobile device screen time. For both outcomes, the ORs for difficulties increased when the children were exposed to both high mobile device screen time and high TV/PC screen time, but the test for effect modification did not support departure from additivity.

Compared to children with less than one hour mobile device screen time who were read to on a daily or almost daily basis, children exposed to either infrequent reading (OR = 1.52; 95% CI: 1.27, 1.82) or high mobile device screen time (OR = 2.13; 95% CI: 1.95, 2.32) were at higher risk of language comprehension difficulties. When exposed to both high mobile device screen time and infrequent reading, the OR for language difficulties increased markedly to 3.49 (95% CI: 3.03, 4.02) for language comprehension difficulties. The test for effect modification supported departure from additivity (SI = 1.51; 95% CI: 1.17, 1.93), with the OR of combined exposure of high mobile device screen time and infrequent reading being more than additive. For expressive language skills difficulties, the ORs were 1.55 (95% CI: 1.30, 1.84) when exposed to high mobile device screen time only and 1.74 (95% CI: 1.60, 1.90) when exposed to infrequent reading only. Combined, the OR for expressive language skills difficulties increased, but the test for effect modification did not support departure from additivity.

Table 5 Adjusted odds ratios and synergy indexes (95% CI) for language comprehension and expressive language skills difficulties by combination of mobile device screen time and factors related to the domestic learning environment

Discussion

The current study suggests that high mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language comprehension and expressive language skills among two- and three-year-old children. Children with mobile device screen time of one hour or more had significantly poorer language development scores on both outcomes compared to children with no mobile device screen time. Likewise, higher odds for being at risk of language comprehension and expressive language skills difficulties were found even after adjustment for sociodemographic characteristics and factors related to the home environment. This confirms the significance of the findings in terms of indication of significant difficulties relative to the majority of children in the same age and gender group. However, mobile device use of less than one hour a day was not associated with poorer language development on any of the two language development measures. Neither was it associated with increased odds for risk of language comprehension and expressive language skills difficulties, respectively. Finally, the results indicate that one aspect of the domestic learning environment– reading frequently to the child– may be a moderating factor that partly buffers the negative effect of high mobile device screen time on language comprehension. However, no modifying effect of parental education and time spent by the child on TV/PC was found.

Our results are in accordance with findings of previous studies examining the association between overall screen time and language development [13, 48] and with a recent longitudinal study that found high screen use in early childhood to be associated with poorer language and cognitive development in kindergarden [49]. Contrary to the findings of the recent meta-analysis by Jing et al. [21], our study found poorer language development scores and higher risks of both language comprehension difficulties and expressive language skills difficulties among 2- and 3-year olds with a high mobile device screen time. There may be a number of reasons for the difference in findings. First, the overall meta-analysis by Jing et al. [21] included both experimental studies and studies conducted in a naturalistic setting. When moderator analyses were conducted, only experimental studies (testing the effect of screen media with educational content) and studies with naturalistic exposure to educational content showed a positive association between media exposure and vocabulary. No significant association was found for studies conducted in ‘real world’ settings where a greater diversity in screen content and quality can be expected. Secondly, a stronger effect was found when studies assessed learning of specific words taught as part of the experiment rather than assessing general vocabulary. Finally, both Jing et al. [21] and Madigan et al. [13] found that younger children benefit less from screen media exposure. Our findings may therefore partly be due to the younger age of the children comprised in our sample (mean age: 31 months) compared to the meta-analysis by Jing et al. [21] (mean age experimental setting: 49 months; naturalistic setting: 33 months). In relation to the association between mobile device screen time, specifically and language development, our findings are consistent with the study of van den Heuvel et al. [9] who found negative associations between mobile device screen time and expressive language among 18-month-old children. Likewise with Moon et al. [20], who found negative associations between mobile device screen time and expressive language among three-year-olds. However, contrary to our study, Moon et al. [20] found no association with language comprehension, and van den Heuvel [9] found no association between mobile device screen time and other communication delays (social and symbolic communication including for example gestures and understanding) among 18-month-olds. In addition, another study by Lin et al. [25] found no association between time spent on mobile devices and overall language delay (combining language and communication skills) among 18- to 36-month-olds. Research in the area is thus often inconsistent, which is most likely due to different measures and operationalizations of both screen use and language development. Thus, especially language comprehension has been operationalized in various ways and additionally, the mode of data collection differed regarding whether completion of the questionnaire was supported by a professional [20, 25] or not. Likewise, the operationalization of mobile device screen time also differed with regard to the questions being asked, and how the response categories were presented and coded. Therefore, differences between the results of the present study and previous studies focusing specifically on mobile device screen time may be partly due to issues related to measurement and operationalization. Additionally, cultural issues related to the child’s environment may have impacted results from different countries; in some environments there may for example be moderating factors not accounted for.

Potential mechanisms

Some potential mechanisms may contribute to that high mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language comprehension and expressive language skills. One mechanism could be time spent on mobile devices may replace time that could be spent on parent-child interaction and language enhancing activities [7, 23, 50]. In addition, the content that young children engage with on mobile devices vary in quality and may in some cases be age-inappropriate [13, 21]. Another mechanism could be that some parents may perceive educational apps, for example, as an alternative to more traditional learning activities which are more beneficial for language development [51, 52]. However, this potential mechanism may be less likely as Levine et al. [53] find that parents with an educational motive for letting their toddler spend time on mobile devices are more likely to co-view the content. If children with high screen time miss out on important communication opportunities with parents or significant others such as siblings, these missed communication opportunities may affect the children’s language development negatively [29, 54].

One way to support parent-child interaction and child language development is through shared book reading, which provides opportunities for dialogue and elaborations of concepts and events [26]. Additionally, shared book reading provides the child with an opportunity to explore the story line and rehearse new words at her or his own pace. In line with this, we found that frequent reading to the child may act as a buffer by reducing some of the negative effect of high mobile device screen time on language comprehension difficulties. However, no modifying effect of reading was found for expressive language skills difficulties. This may be due to the fact that reading for some parents is a one-way communication in which the child listens but only to a limited degree express words itself [31].

Contrary to our expectations, two other factors related to the domestic learning environment– parental education and time the child spent on watching TV or PC– did not modify the association between high mobile device screen time and language development. This suggests two independent effects: mobile device screen time and parental education appear to affect language development independently of one another; and mobile device screen time and TV/PC time appear to affect language development independently of one another. That is, the OR of the combined exposures did not exceed the sum of the separate ORs.

Strength and limitations

A major strength of the present study is the large, close-to-representative sample of two- and three-year-old children that provides data on mobile device screen time, child language development, parental characteristics and the child’s home environment. This made it possible to adjust for important confounders such as parental mental well-being, parental preoccupation with smartphone and frequency of reading to the child, and to examine potential modifiers of the association. Secondly, measuring mobile device screen time and time spent on TV/PC separately enabled us to study the association between language development and mobile device screen time specifically. To our knowledge, only few studies have studied this association despite the widespread use of mobile devices among toddlers and despite the shift towards use of mobile devices instead of TV and PCs (which most research focus on) during the last decade [13].

There are, however, also a number of important limitations in the present study. First, the cross-sectional design of the study does not reveal the direction of the association between mobile device screen time and language development. Secondly, both mobile device screen time and language development were parent reported. This may have caused screen time to be underreported if some parents wanted to provide a more socially desirable answer. However, parents were informed that their response to the web-based questionnaire was treated as strictly confidential before completing the questionnaire. Thirdly, assessment of language development is complex. Therefore, the FTF-Toddlers questionnaire cannot be used for diagnostic determination of language problems since such diagnostic determination should always be conducted by a professional. Also, detailed information on language development such as vocabulary and grammar were not available, which may have given us a less nuanced picture of children’s language development. Still, parents experience their child in various contexts. Since the FTF-Toddlers subscales are based on these parental experiences, the subscales do provide information on parent-reported difficulties within language development. We did not find any systematic differences in the findings of previous studies on mobile device screen time and language development [9, 20, 25], depending on outcome measure nor whether language development was parent reported or directly assessed by a professional. Another limitation is that the association between mobile device screen time and language development may depend on the content quality, degree of co-viewing and interaction about the content. For example, co-viewing has been stressed as important for young children, because they rely on interaction with their adult caregivers in order to transfer learning achieved from screens to real-world settings [7, 16]. Likewise, the effect of mobile device screen use may depend on whether the content is age appropriate and of an educational character [13, 21]. Also, children whose parents let their children use mobile devices for non-educational purposes may be more likely to spend time alone on mobile devices [53]. While the need for more knowledge of co-viewing and content quality has been highlighted [9, 13], we did not have information about these factors in the present study. Finally, frequency of reading to the child was included as a potential confounder in the analyses of the association between mobile device screen time and language development. However, it is possible that frequency of reading is a mediator between screen time and language development, that is, that parents spend less time reading to their child because the child asks for and spends time on mobile devices. If so, including reading as a confounder would be an over adjustment and the associations of the present study would be underestimated.

Conclusions and implications

Mobile devices have become an integral part of everyday life and they are now ubiquitous in the lives of even very young children. This study found that one hour or more of mobile device screen time a day was significantly associated with poorer language development scores and higher risks of both language comprehension difficulties and expressive language skills difficulties among two- and three-year-old children. The results remained significant after adjustment for sociodemographic and home environment characteristics. A buffering effect of reading to the child was found on language comprehension difficulties, while parental education and child TV/PC time did not modify the associations with language comprehension difficulties or with expressive language difficulties.

Since the study is cross-sectional, further research using longitudinal data is needed in order to determine the causality of our findings. However, our results point out significant associations between high mobile device use and poorer language development, which in itself is associated with socio-emotional, educational and cognitive outcomes [1,2,3,4, 55]. The knowledge of this study is therefore of importance to both parents, child specialists and clinicians as it illuminates the potential risks of providing young children with mobile devices to a degree that it may negatively affect their language development.

Data availability

Data used in this study are not publicly available as they are stored on the secure servers of Statistics Denmark. Due to privacy concerns, the data cannot be made available outside the hosted research servers at Statistics Denmark. Provided that researchers are affiliated with a research environments preapproved by Statistics Denmark, data are available on reasonable request sent to the corresponding author.

References

  1. Curtis PR, Frey JR, Watson CD, Hampton LH, Roberts MY. Language disorders and problem behaviors: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018;142(2).

  2. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Pathways to reading: the role of oral language in the transition to reading. Dev Psychol. 2005;41(2):428–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Johnson CJ, Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB. Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2010;19:51–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Raviv T, Kessenich M, Morrison FJ. A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: the role of parenting factors. Early Child Res Q. 2004;19(4):528–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Harrington A. Screen time use in children under 3 years old: a systematic review of correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Haughton C, Aiken M, Cheevers C. Cyber babies: the impact of emerging technology on the developing infant. Psychol Res. 2015;5(9):504–18.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Taylor G, Monaghan P, Westermann G. Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. J Child Media. 2018;12(1):51–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. van den Heuvel M, Ma J, Borkhoff CM, Koroshegyi C, Dai DWH, Parkin PC, et al. Mobile media device use is associated with expressive language delay in 18-month-old children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(2):99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rideout V. The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Elias N, Sulkin I. Screen-assisted parenting: the relationship between toddlers’ screen time and parents’ use of media as a parenting tool. J Fam Issues. 2019;40(18):2801–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Christakis DA. The effects of infant media usage: what do we know and what should we learn? Acta Paediatr. 2009;98(1):8–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM, Xu D, et al. Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Huberman HS, Alvir J, et al. Infant television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeconomic status households. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(5):411–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Barr R. Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: informing theory and practice. Dev Rev. 2010;30(2):128–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Kildare CA, Middlemiss W. Impact of parents mobile device use on parent-child interaction: a literature review. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;75:579–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Associations between screen-based media use and brain white matter integrity in preschool-aged children. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):e193869–193869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Asikainen M, Kylliäinen A, Mäkelä TE, Saarenpää-Heikkilä O, Paavonen EJ. Exposure to electronic media was negatively associated with speech and language development at 18 and 24 months. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(11):3046–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Moon J, Cho SY, Lim SM, Roh JH, Koh MS, Kim YJ, et al. Smart device usage in early childhood is differentially associated with fine motor and language development. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108(5):903–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Jing M, Ye T, Kirkorian HL, Mares M. Screen media exposure and young children’s vocabulary learning and development: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2023;94(5):1398–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Linebarger DL, Vaala SE. Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: an ecological perspective. Dev Rev. 2010;30(2):176–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Radesky JS, Christakis DA. Increased screen time: implications for early childhood development and behavior. Pediatr Clin. 2016;63(5):827–39.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Domoff SE, Radesky JS, Harrison K, Riley H, Lumeng JC, Miller AL. A naturalistic study of child and family screen media and mobile device use. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(2):401–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Lin H-P, Chen K-L, Chou W, Yuan K-S, Yen S-Y, Chen Y-S, et al. Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers. Infant Behav Dev. 2020;58:101424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Dowdall N, Melendez-Torres GJ, Murray L, Gardner F, Hartford L, Cooper PJ. Shared picture book reading interventions for child language development: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Child Dev. 2020;91(2):e383–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Sundqvist A, Koch F-S, Birberg Thornberg U, Barr R, Heimann M. Growing up in a digital world–digital media and the association with the child’s language development at two years of age. Front Psychol. 2021;12:569920.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Pace A, Luo R, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annu Rev Linguist. 2017;3:285–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Määttä S, Kaukonen R, Vepsäläinen H, Lehto E, Ylönen A, Ray C, et al. The mediating role of the home environment in relation to parental educational level and preschool children’s screen time: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Weigel DJ, Lowman JL, Martin SS. Language development in the years before school: a comparison of developmental assets in home and child care settings. Early Child Dev Care. 2007;177(6–7):719–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. VIVE The Danish Center for Social Science Research. SPOR (In English TRACES) – Children’s development and wellbeing throughout life [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2022 Jul 28]. https://www.vive.dk/en/themes/project-spor/.

  33. VIVE The Danish Center for Social Science Reseach. The questionnaire survey [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2022 Jul 28]. https://www.vive.dk/en/themes/project-spor/the-questionnaire-survey/.

  34. Kadesjö B, Miniscalo C, Hagberg B, Haavisto A, Gillberg. Nordisk spørgeskema til vurdering af børns udvikling og adfærd. Småbørnsversion – for børn i alderen 2–5 år [Questionnaire for evaluation of development and behavior in 2–5 year old children] [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2021 Oct 27]. Available from: www.5-15.org/download.

  35. Kadesjö B, Janois L-O, Korkman M, Mickelsson K, Strand G, Trillingsgaard A et al. Five-To-Fifteen-Revised (5-15R) [Internet]. 2017. Available from: www.5-15.org.

  36. Lambek R, Trillingsgaard A. Elaboration, validation and standardization of the five to fifteen (FTF) questionnaire in a Danish population sample. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;38:161–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Schachinger-Lorentzon U, Kadesjö B, Gillberg C, Miniscalco C. Children screening positive for language delay at 2.5 years: Language Disorder and developmental profiles. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:3267.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Hill E, Calder S, Candy C, Truscott G, Kaur J, Savage B et al. Low language capacity in childhood: a systematic review of prevalence estimates. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2023.

  39. Raaska H, Elovainio M, Sinkkonen J, Stolt S, Jalonen I, Matomäki J, et al. Adopted children’s language difficulties and their relation to symptoms of reactive attachment disorder: FinAdo study. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2013;34(3):152–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Statistics Denmark. Immigrants in Denmark [in Danish: Indvandrere i Danmark]. Copenhagen; 2008.

  41. Topp CW, Østergaard SD, Søndergaard S, Bech P. The WHO-5 well-being index: a systematic review of the literature. Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Northside Partnership Doyle O, Team UGIPE. Preparing for Life collection: Evaluation of the Preparing for Life early childhood intervention, 2008–2015. [collection]. Version 1. Dublin; 2017.

  43. Vandenbroucke JP, von Elm E, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Mulrow CD, Pocock SJ, et al. Strengthening the reporting of Observational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. Int J Surg. 2014;12(12):1500–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. World Health Organization. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. World Health Organization; 2019.

  45. American Academy of Pediatrics. Children and media tips from the American Academy of Pediatrics. 2018.

  46. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern epidemiology. Volume 3. Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Philadelphia; 2008.

  47. Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Confidence interval estimation of interaction. Epidemiology. 1992;452–6.

  48. McArthur BA, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen time and developmental and behavioral outcomes for preschool children. Pediatr Res. 2022;91(6):1616–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Vanderloo LM, Janus M, Omand JA, Keown-Stoneman CDG, Borkhoff CM, Duku E, et al. Children’s screen use and school readiness at 4–6 years: prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. McArthur BA, Eirich R, McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen use relates to decreased offline enrichment activities. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(3):896–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Radesky JS, Eisenberg S, Kistin CJ, Gross J, Block G, Zuckerman B, et al. Overstimulated consumers or next-generation learners? Parent tensions about child mobile technology use. Ann Fam Med. 2016;14(6):503–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Brown A, Smolenaers E. Parents’ interpretations of screen time recommendations for children younger than 2 years. J Fam Issues. 2018;39(2):406–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Levine LE, Waite BM, Bowman LL, Kachinsky K. Mobile media use by infants and toddlers. Comput Hum Behav. 2019;94:92–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L, Melstein Damast A. Responsive parenting in the second year: specific influences on children’s language and play. Early Dev Parent Int J Res Pract. 1996;5(4):173–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Language development and intelligence in midlife. Br J Dev Psychol. 2019;37(2):269–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Legal Information. Act on Research Ethics Review of Health Research Projects. 2017. (In danish: Bekendtgørelse af lov om videnskabsetisk behandling af sundhedsvidenskabelige forskningsprojekter, LBK nr 1083 [Internet]. 2017. https://www.retsinformation.dk/eli/lta/2017/1083.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participating families of the TRACES survey for their participation.

Funding

This research was supported by grant number NNF17SH0026396 from the Novo Nordisk Foundation. The funder’s role was solely funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SBR conceptualized and designed the study, carried out the analyses, drafted the initial manuscript and reviewed and revised the manuscript. GTO and TFM contributed to the study design, discussed the results and reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Signe Boe Rayce.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate

According to Danish law, register- and survey based studies do not require approval from the National Committee for Health Research Ethics. The notification exemption of survey- and register based studies is described in part 4, Sect. 14 (2) of the Danish Act on Research Ethics Review of Health Research Projects (56). The study complies with the Declaration of Helsinki and was registered with the Danish Data Protection Authority. All parents of children participating in TRACES received written information about the survey, were informed that participation was voluntary, that data are treated confidentially and that storage and analyses of data comply with GDPR rules. The parents’ voluntary completion and submission of the survey constituted implied informed consent. TRACES has been evaluated and approved by the Danish Center for Social Science Research’s Internal Research Council.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rayce, S.B., Okholm, G.T. & Flensborg-Madsen, T. Mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language development among toddlers: results from a large-scale survey. BMC Public Health 24, 1050 (2024). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-024-18447-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-024-18447-4

Keywords