Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates real-world behavioural response to the food retail environment in children

Abstract

Background

Evidence for the impact of the food retailing environment on food-related and obesity outcomes remains equivocal, but only a few studies have attempted to identify sub-populations for whom this relationship might be stronger than others. Genetic polymorphisms related to dopamine signalling have been associated with differences in responses to rewards such as food and may be candidate markers to identify such sub-populations. This study sought to investigate whether genetic variation of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4 exon III 48 bp VNTR polymorphism) moderated the association between local exposure to food retailers on BMI and diet in a sample of 4 to12-year-old children.

Methods

Data collected from a birth cohort and a community cross-sectional study conducted in Montreal, Canada, were combined to provide DRD4 VNTR polymorphism data in terms of presence of the 7-repeat allele (DRD4-7R) for 322 children aged between 4 and 12 (M (SD): 6.8(2.8) y). Outcomes were Body Mass Index (BMI) for age and energy density derived from a Food Frequency Questionnaire. Food environment was expressed as the proportion of local food retailers classified as healthful within 3 km of participants’ residence. Linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, income, cohort, and geographic clustering were used to test gene*environment interactions.

Results

A significant gene*food environment interaction was found for energy density with results indicating that DRD4-7R carriers had more energy dense diets than non-carriers, with this effect being more pronounced in children living in areas with proportionally more unhealthy food retailers. No evidence of main or interactive effects of DRD4 VNTR and food environment was found for BMI.

Conclusions

Results of the present study suggest that a genetic marker related to dopamine pathways can identify children with potentially greater responsiveness to unhealthy local food environment. Future studies should investigate additional elements of the food environment and test whether results hold across different populations.

Peer Review reports

Background

Major global shifts in the quantity and quality of food available through our food supply have been linked to the increasing trends in obesity worldwide [1]. Variations in food supply at the neighbourhood level and their link to disparities in nutrition and nutrition-related outcomes have also been extensively investigated. The evidence emerging from this body of work has however failed to provide strong evidence supporting the impact of the neighbourhood food environment on food behaviour and related cardio-metabolic risk factors, with many studies reporting null or counter-intuitive findings [2,3,4]. Although the equivocal evidence could be explained by methodological limitations and heterogeneity in studies [2, 5,6,7], results could also reflect the fact that the local food environment does influence some people, but not all. This idea is consistent with calls for moving away from dichotomous understanding of neighbourhood effects in terms of whether or not they do matter to understanding of how and for whom they matter [8, 9].

While neuroscience studies have assessed how individuals respond to various food cues in controlled experimental settings [10,11,12], such work has been sparsely conducted in naturalistic settings. Exceptions include a study in which food consumption in children with a greater predisposition to external eating (eating in response to external cues) was found to be more strongly related to exposure to vegetable and soft drink displays compared to children with lower external eating [13]. Another example is a study showing that neighbourhood exposure to fast-food restaurants was a stronger predictor of the frequency of visits to fast food retailers in adults considered to be more reward-sensitive [14]. Another study found that an inverse association between one’s sense of mastery, a construct of perceived control over one’s circumstances, and risk for metabolic syndrome, was more pronounced for individuals living in areas with greater exposure to fast-food restaurants [15].

Further insights into individual differences in responses to food environment can be gained by looking into the neurobiology of behavioural responses to environmental cues. From a neurobehavioural perspective, environmental reactivity results from the interaction between the characteristics or traits of individuals and the valence and strength of the environmental stimulus they are exposed to [16]. With respect to neurobiological traits, main candidates include genetic and brain markers of the dopamine neurotransmitter systems, which is responsible for modulating the functioning of neural circuitry at any time, and in any environmental context, with complementary role played by the serotonin system [17]. Dopaminergic neurons are especially important at encoding motivational value and supporting brain networks for orienting, cognition, and general motivation [17]. It is recognised that the dopamine system plays an important role in the rewarding processes underlying food intake [18, 19] and if compromised, can lead to food addiction and obesity [20]. Polymorphisms in genes that significantly affect dopamine neurotransmission are therefore potential candidate genetic markers for food environment responsiveness. In particular, the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) functionally produces inhibitory effects, and is expressed in brain regions related to planning and regulating executive functions and rewards [21]. The most frequently studied D4 polymorphisms is the 48-bp variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) on the exon 3 of the DRD4 gene, which is repeated from 2 to 11 times, with the 7-repeat allele being associated with altered function of the receptor relative to the more common 4-repeat allele [22]. The 7-repeat (7R) allele is associated with a lower affinity for dopamine and thus, reduced inhibitory effects on postsynaptic neurons [23,24,25], leading to increased sensitivity to both aversive and rewarding cues. Children who are carriers of this allele have also been found to have less healthy food consumption patterns compared to non-carriers [22, 26].

The vulnerability to adverse or unhealthy environments described above, also known as the diathesis-stress view, represents only one side of variation in responsiveness to environment. It has been suggested that differences in dopaminergic pathway could lead individuals to respond more intensely to not only adverse (e.g. obesogenic) conditions, but also supportive (health-promoting) environments [27], translating into the well-established “for-better–and-for-worse” pattern of susceptibility [28]. This hypothesis known as the differential susceptibility hypothesis [16, 29] suggests that individual variations in the magnitude of biological responses regulate openness or susceptibility to environmental influences, ranging from particularly harmful to favourable environments [29].

The DRD4-7R VNTR occupies a central place among genetic polymorphisms associated with differential susceptibility and other forms of behavioural plasticity [27]. For instance, evidence of differential susceptibility has been reported for cognitive and socio-emotional child development outcomes of family conditions [21], sensitivity to depression and associated intervention [30, 31], as well as sensitivity to cultural norms [32, 33]. As early evidence supporting the possibility of differential susceptibility in the domain of obesity and other diet-related health problems, Silveira and colleagues [34] found that children who were carriers of the DRD4-7R VNTR and raised under adverse low-socioeconomic status (SES), showed a stronger preference for fat compared to the control non-carriers group. On the other hand, DRD4-7R VNTR carriers raised in high-SES conditions reported lower preference for fatty foods compared to the non-carriers, supporting a “for better and for worse” pattern. A recent study also provided evidence for differential susceptibility to emotional eating in response to a positive postnatal environment according to a predicted gene expression of the prefrontal DRD4 gene [35].

Although socio-economic conditions and disadvantage have been linked to obesogenic qualities of the food environment [36], the above studies did not investigate directly the food environment. The present study was designed to explicitly investigate whether the association between a measure of adverse (obesogenic) vs. healthy food environment and dietary behaviour and obesity vary by DRD4-7R VNTR status in children and if so, whether differences in pattern of responsiveness are consistent with the diathesis-stress or the differential susceptibility hypotheses.

Methods

Data for this study were obtained from two studies. The first study is the Brain-to-Society (BtS) diagnostic research project, a cross-sectional study conducted in 2013 examining a range of individual, household, and environmental risk factors for childhood obesity in 6–12 year children living in Montreal, Canada. The second study is the Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability and Neurodevelopment (MAVAN) birth cohort which recruited pregnant women from obstetric clinics in hospitals located in Montréal, Québec and Hamilton, Ontario from 2003 onwards, with the present study only using data from the Montreal cohort and assessments made when children were 4 years old.

BtS recruitment and data collection

Recruitment for the BtS study was conducted by an independent research firm from an existing database of households known to (1) live in the Montreal Metropolitan area, (2) likely have children in the target age group (6-12y), and (3) have expressed a willingness to participate in academic research. From this database, 4947 households were randomly selected and contacted by phone. Contacted individuals asked about their willingness to complete a survey about children’s eating and lifestyle habits. Eligibility was confirmed eligibility for 1149 individuals, of those 616 completed the interviews. The response rate was estimated at 23% using a formula that allows for the potential eligibility of a proportion of respondents with unknown eligibility. Each participating household answered questions for one eligible child. If more than one child was eligible, the child with the next birthday was selected. Questions regarding the selected child were answered by parent/guardian with the best knowledge of the child’s daily habits. Interviews were conducted between March and August 2013 and had a duration of 50 min. Verbal consent was obtained from all participants prior to starting the phone survey and a written consent for follow-up data collections. Ethical approval covering all aspects of the research was obtained from McGill University’s Institutional Research Board.

Participants who completed the interview were invited to complete a self-administered questionnaire, which included a link to a validated web-based Food-Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) [37]. Participants received questionnaires and consent (parent) and assent (child) forms by mail and returned them upon completion using a pre-paid envelope.

The FFQ asked participants to recall their children’s food intake over the last month based on a list of 136 individual food items or food clusters covering eight food and beverages categories including dairy products, vegetables, fruits, meat and alternatives, beverages, cereal and grain products, other foods, and supplements. Participants were asked to report the frequency of consumption of each food item on a 8- or 9-point scale from “never” to “four or more times per day” using portion sizes estimated using digital photographs of the food in standardized dinnerware and utensils based on the SU. VI. MAX food atlas [38]. The Nutrition Data System for Research [39] and the Canadian Nutrient file [40, 41] were used to calculate the energy content (calories) and the nutrient intake. Height and weight were reported by parents. At the end of the initial telephone survey, parents/guardians were asked if they would participate in a genetic sub-study by providing a saliva sample from the child and/or completing a mailed (or on-line) follow-up questionnaire. The kit was mailed to the families with written instructions on how to collect the sample, and then returned using a pre-paid envelope for DNA extraction at the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health Neurogenetics Laboratory (CAMH, University of Toronto).

MAVAN recruitment and data collection

MAVAN recruitment was conducted in obstetric clinics in hospitals where mothers were invited from mid-pregnancy. Inclusion criteria included being 18 years of age or older, and fluent in either English or French. Exclusion criteria included serious obstetric complications during the pregnancy or delivery of the child, extreme low birth weight, prematurity (≤37 weeks’ gestation), or any congenital diseases. Dyads of mothers and their offspring were assessed longitudinally at home and in the laboratory across the child’s development (3, 6, 12, 18, 24, 36, 48, 60 and 72 months); however, this study is using data collected at 48 months of age, a time point when the FFQ was administered. The project received approvals from obstetricians from study hospitals, ethics committees and university affiliates. All participants provided informed consent. For more information on MAVAN methodology and measures, see [42].

The 48-month assessment, involved a number of laboratory assessments about the children, including food-related measures and height and weight measures. Standing height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm without shoes with a stadiometer (PE-AIM-101;Perspective Enterprises) and body weight was measured in light clothing to the nearest 0.1 kg with a digital floor scale Tanita. Mothers completed FFQs valid for the local population [43] for their child, while the child performed behavioural tasks and measurements. Mothers were asked to report their child’s intake of various foods on a typical day in the last week, with the aid of a food and measures photo album to estimate the portion size of each food [44]. Total caloric and macronutrient intake was calculated using the NutriBase software (version NB7; CyberSoft Inc). Saliva samples were collected for genotyping.

Outcome measures

Were (1) body mass index for age and sex calculated from parent-reported or directly measured children’s height and weight, in z-scores according to WHO growth standards (WHO, 2006), which was available for 544 children across the two samples; (2) eating behaviour as indexed by energy density calculated as the ratio of the total energy intake (in kcal) to the total daily weight of food and beverages consumed (g). These measures were selected to reflect the expected behavioural and physiological impacts of food environments promoting high-fat, high-sugar food choices. In addition, given that the study focused on genetic variability in responses to palatable food, we selected energy density as our behavioural measure due to its established correlation with the palatability and rewarding value of food [45, 46].

Genotyping

For both samples, genomic DNA was extracted using a high-capacity membrane-based column (QuickGene810, AutoGen, Inc., Holliston, MA) and was quantitated using an A260/A280 ratio with a NanoDrop spectrophotometer (ThermoScientific, Inc., Wilmington, DE) and agarose gel electrophoresis. The DRD4 VNTR polymorphism was amplified, with 0.2 μM of DRD4 forward primer 5′-GCGACTACGTGGTCTACTCG and 0.2 μM of DRD4 reverse primer 5′-AGGACCCTCATGGCCTTG [47], using the Roche GC-Rich PCR System amplification buffer (Roche Applied Science, Inc., Mannheim, Germany) and 20 ng of genomic DNA in a volume of 25 μl. A Stratagene thermocycler (Life Technologies, Inc., Grand Island, NY) was used to heat the samples at 95 °C for 3 min, then cycled 40 times at 95 °C for 20 s, 57 °C for 20 s, and 72 °C for 1 min, followed by 72 °C for 3 min. Polymerase chain reaction products were separated and visualized on a 2% agarose gel (type 1-A, Sigma, St. Louis, MO) stained with ethidium bromide (Lichter, Barr et al. 1993). DNA genotyping was performed blind to the children’s behaviour and phenotype.

Characterization of individually unique food environment

Individual local food retailing environment was measured using the modified Retail Food Environment Index (mRFEI)), which represents the proportion of local food retailers classified as healthful and was originally developed by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) [48]. We selected a relative measure of the retail food environment index over an absolute measure of availability of specific types of food stores in light of evidence that such indices are more robustly related to health outcomes than absolute availability measures [2]. Local food retailers included were supermarkets, grocery stores, fruit and vegetable stores, supercentres, convenience stores and fast-food restaurants located within 3 km of participants’ residence. As per previous the CDC definition [48], the following retailers were categorised as providing healthy food: supermarkets, grocery stores, fruit and vegetable stores, supercentres. The mRFEI was computed as the number of healthy food retailers over the total number of local food retailers using kernel density estimates allowing weighing retailers based on their proximity to the participant’s residence.

Analyses

The Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium of the DRD4-7R variant was assessed using a Chi-square test with one degree of freedom. Generalized linear models accounting for spatial clustering by Census Tract through the use of Generalised Estimating Equations (GEE) estimation were used to separately model each outcome measured as a function of mRFEI, genotype, and their interaction. Analyses were conducted on the maximal available sample. Models were estimated using the GENMOD Procedure in SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). All analyses were adjusted for children’s age (in years), cohort (BtS or MAVAN), sex, and household income above or below a low-income cut-off. Low income cut-offs used in the BtS study was 45,000 CAD, which was the closest cut-off value to the low-income cut-offs (before tax) for 4-person households in large communities (> 500,000 population) when the data were collected in 2013 (44,340$). For MAVAN, low income cut-offs were based on Statistics Canada Low-Income Cut-offs Index (LICO) [49]. Ethnicity was not available in the BtS sample and was therefore not included in analyses. Statistical significance was set at alpha = 0.05. Probing and testing of measures to dissociate differential susceptibility from diathesis-stress were informed by approaches proposed by Roisman and colleagues [50] and Widaman and colleagues [51] and conducted using an online calculator [52]. These measures are described in Additional file 1.

Results

Sample description

Genetic information was available for 322 participants, of those 305 had food environment data and 300 also had household income information. Of those, BMI and energy density data were available for 279 and 230 participants, respectively. Descriptive characteristics for participants included in the BMI analytical sample, from both cohorts are presented in Table 1. The distribution of the DRD4-7R VNTR allele was in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (Chi-square = 0.0006, p = 0.98) in the sample. Both samples were similar in terms of sex representation, genotype, outcomes, and food environment measure. MAVAN cohort participants were younger, as expected, and had a higher prevalence of low income. No differences were found in mRFEI based on DRD4 7R VNTR status (p > 0.8).

Table 1 Participant characteristics included in both analytic samples by study (BtS/MAVAN) and combined

Gene*food environment results

Main and interactive effects of DRD4-7R VNTR status and the food environment index on energy density and zBMI are provided in Table 2. Results for energy density revealed main effects of DRD4-7R VNTR, with the presence of the DRD4-7R VNTR being associated with greater energy density. No main effect of the food environment was found for either outcome. A statistically significant interaction effect was found between genotyping and the mRFEI measure on energy density, which suggested that there was an inverse association between mRFEI and energy density for DRD4-7R VNTR carriers only. Results for zBMI suggest no significant main or interactive effects of DRD4-7R VNTR and food environment measures.

Table 2 Results of DRD4-7R VNTR *Food environment interaction for BtS-MAVAN Combined (models adjusted for age (in years), sex, low household income status (< 45 K), and geographic clustering by Census tract)

We explored the nature of the interaction found between mRFEI and DRD4-7R VNTR for energy density using probing and measures allowing to assess whether the interaction was consistent with the differential susceptibility or diathesis-stress hypothesis as described elsewhere [50, 51] and in Additional file 1. To do so, we first tested the assumption of linearity of relationships as suggested by Widaman and colleagues [51] by testing for quadratic and cubic terms for centred values of mRFEI within each DRD4-7R VNTR status group. None of the non-linear terms were statistically significant (P’s > 0.5). The cross-over point, that is, the value of the mRFEI where the regression lines for DRD4-7R VNTR carriers and non-carriers cross, was computed and the proportion of the sample with values of mRFEI above the cross-over point was estimated at 14.5%, suggesting that the cross-over point occurred at the higher end (i.e. healthier end) of the mRFEI range observed. This measure, known as the Proportion Affected (PA) index represents an estimate of the proportion of the population that is differentially affected by the moderator (DRD4 7R VNTR), with values closer to 0.5 considered to provide support for the differential susceptibility hypothesis whereas values closer to 0 considered to be more consistent with the diathesis-stress hypothesis (Roisman, Newman [50]).

The cross-over point at high values of mRFEI was confirmed by plotting and probing the relationship between mRFEI and energy density for DRD4-7R VNTR carriers and non-carriers (Fig. 1). This analysis also confirmed that an inverse association between mRFEI and energy density was only present for DRD4-7R VNTR carriers, as shown in the figure. The figure also illustrates in grey the values of mRFEI for which there was a statistically significant difference in energy density according to DRD4-7R VNTR status, which suggests that differences were only present for relatively less healthy food environments, with differences in energy density between carriers and non-carriers being approximately 0.3 SD (approximately 0.2 kcal/g) for children living in areas of average mRFEI value and 0.6 SD (0.4 kcal/g) for those in areas 1 SD below the mean (i.e. more unhealthy than average). Finally, the figure also highlights that the area for which carriers are more negatively affected by the environment than non-carriers (labelled as ‘w’ for worse) is greater than the area for which they are advantaged by the environment (‘b’ for better). Overall, the results appear to be more consistent with the diathesis-stress hypothesis than the differential susceptibility hypothesis.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Associations between mRFEI and energy density for DRD4-7R VNTR carriers and non-carriers. Shaded area represents the area for which energy density differed between carriers and non-carriers, ‘w’ and ‘b’ indicates areas where carriers are respectively worse or better than non-carriers

Discussion

Our data provide evidence of individual genetic differences in the behavioural impact of the commercial food environment, supporting the moderating effect of the DRD4-7R VNTR polymorphism. While results revealed that DRD4-7R carriers had overall more energy dense diets than non-carriers, this effect was more pronounced for children living in areas with proportionally more unhealthy food retailers. Further investigation of the pattern of interaction suggested that results were consistent with a diathesis-stress pattern, with no evidence of children who were carriers of the DRD4-7R VNTR polymorphism being more positively affected by healthy food environments than non-carriers, as would be expected under the differential susceptibility pattern. Moreover, evidence of association between the food retail environment and behaviour was only found for DRD4-7R VNTR carriers.

The lack of evidence for differential susceptibility contrasts with other studies conducted investigating the DRD4-7R VNTR polymorphism. In particular, a study by Silveira and colleagues [34] conducted in the MAVAN sample provided evidence of differential susceptibility to socio-economic conditions in relation to food-related outcomes, although the focus on fat preference rather than total energy density intake may explain the differences considering that variations in the dopamine signalling may be particularly and specifically linked to variations in the intake of palatable foods [26]. Another reason for the observed differences might be that the benefits associated with favourable socio-economic conditions may not depend on more healthful food sources, but perhaps more on factors such as better food education and parental support.

In contrast to the energy density results, no main or interactive effects of gene and food environment were found for zBMI. This null finding, however, does not invalidate the results for energy density as they can be due to a number of reasons. For example, dopamine pathways and DRD4 polymorphisms have also been related to physical activity levels in animals and humans [53], which may counteract the metabolic effect of a high-energy diet. Unfortunately, physical activity data were not available to test this explanation. Another potential reason could be the relatively young age of the sample, in which the cumulative impact of energy density on BMI may not have yet taken place. Finally, obesity and its associated markers are influenced by a range of inter-related social, economic, cultural and community factors. Future studies should be undertaken in larger samples while considering a range of obesogenic factors to explore potential mechanisms.

The results need to be interpreted in the context of limitations tied to naturalistic observation and small sample size of the study and they await further replication in both field and laboratory research studies. Statistical power also did not allow testing for sex differences in results, with previous studies indicating sex-specific effects [34]. In addition, our sample combined data from children from pre-school age up to 12 years old, which are likely to differ in their food preferences and level of independence with respect to food decisions. Children within this age range could also be considered to have a limited impact on household food purchasing decisions. However, children have been shown to influence supermarket purchases [54, 55]. Moreover, a study provided evidence that younger children are more likely to ask for advertised food products compared to adolescents [56]. Unfortunately, our sample size did not allow to test whether results differed by age groups. Even though the two data sources sampled children from the same metropolitan region and used the same genetic and environmental measures, the studies differed in their sampling frames, anthropometric measurement methods and food-frequency questionnaires used, which may have introduced additional sources of heterogeneity in the combined data. Ethnicity and education measures were not available in the BtS sample and were therefore not considered in the analyses. Future studies should therefore explore potential age- and ethnicity-related differences and adjust for parental education. In addition, information was not available on the genotype, phenotype or diet of the parents who play an important role in shaping the family food environment, with recent findings suggesting less healthful home food environment in mothers with lower levels of executive functioning (also related to the dopamine system) [57]. Future research is also needed to characterize the neurobiological, behavioural, and psychological endophenotypes associated with the function of the dopamine pathways through which DRD4-7R VNTR susceptibility impacts behaviour (e.g., motivated attention, reward seeking, inhibitory control, and mental flexibility) and using genome-wide approaches. There is a need to trace the multiscale pathways linking genes and environmental factors to outcomes through specific component brain processes, characterizing the neuromodulator foundation of these genetic variables in terms of the basic nature of their reactivity to environmental contexts. Finally, the food environment exposure only considered the type of food outlets available locally and not the variation in foods and marketing strategies used within each outlet and we did not have information on where participants shopped and what they bought. Future studies should consider using sales receipts, sales or loyalty programs data combined with genetic data for a more accurate portrait of purchasing decisions and their genetic and environmental influences.

Conclusion

Results of the present study suggest that a common genetic marker related to dopamine pathways can identify children with greater responsiveness to local food environment. The results confirm the need for food environment research to move beyond determining whether food environment matters, and to investigate for whom it matters and under what conditions in order to develop the best possible strategies to those most likely to benefit from them.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

DRD4-7R VNTR:

7-repeat (7R) allele of the 48-bp variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) on the exon 3 of the D4 receptor (DRD4) gene

zBMI:

Body Mass Index for age expressed in z-scores according to WHO growth standards

MAVAN:

Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability and Neurodevelopment (MAVAN) birth cohort

mRFEI:

modified Retail Food Environment Index

BtS:

Brain-to-Society (BtS) study

References

  1. Hall KD. Did the food environment cause the obesity epidemic? Obesity. 2018;26(1):11–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Cobb LK, Appel LJ, Franco M, Jones-Smith JC, Nur A, Anderson CA. The relationship of the local food environment with obesity: a systematic review of methods, study quality, and results. Obesity. 2015;23(7):1331–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Den Braver N, Lakerveld J, Rutters F, Schoonmade L, Brug J, Beulens J. Built environmental characteristics and diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med. 2018;16(1):12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Wilkins E, Radley D, Morris M, Hobbs M, Christensen A, Marwa WL, et al. A systematic review employing the GeoFERN framework to examine methods, reporting quality and associations between the retail food environment and obesity. Health Place. 2019;57:186–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Lytle LA, Sokol RL. Measures of the food environment: a systematic review of the field, 2007–2015. Health Place. 2017;44:18–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bivoltsis A, Cervigni E, Trapp G, Knuiman M, Hooper P, Ambrosini GL. Food environments and dietary intakes among adults: does the type of spatial exposure measurement matter? A systematic review. Int J Health Geogr. 2018;17(1):19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Gamba RJ, Schuchter J, Rutt C, Seto EYW. Measuring the food environment and its effects on obesity in the United States: a systematic review of methods and results. J Community Health. 2015;40(3):464–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Minh A, Muhajarine N, Janus M, Brownell M, Guhn M. A review of neighborhood effects and early child development: how, where, and for whom, do neighborhoods matter? Health Place. 2017;46:155–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sharkey P, Faber JW. Where, when, why, and for whom do residential contexts matter? Moving away from the dichotomous understanding of neighborhood effects. Annu Rev Sociol. 2014;40:559–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Rapuano KM, Zieselman AL, Kelley WM, Sargent JD, Heatherton TF, Gilbert-Diamond D. Genetic risk for obesity predicts nucleus accumbens size and responsivity to real-world food cues. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114(1):160–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. van der Klaauw AA, von dem Hagen EA, Keogh JM, Henning E, O'Rahilly S, Lawrence AD, et al. Obesity-associated melanocortin-4 receptor mutations are associated with changes in the brain response to food cues. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014;99(10):E2101–E6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Demos KE, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. J Neurosci. 2012;32(16):5549–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Paquet C, de Montigny L, Labban A, Buckeridge D, Ma Y, Arora N, et al. The moderating role of food cue sensitivity in the behavioral response of children to their neighborhood food environment: a cross-sectional study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Paquet C, Daniel M, Knäuper B, Gauvin L, Kestens Y, Dube L. Interactive effects of reward sensitivity and residential fast-food restaurant exposure on fast-food consumption. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91(3):771–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Paquet C, Dubé L, Gauvin L, Kestens Y, Daniel M. Sense of mastery and metabolic risk: moderating role of the local fast-food environment. Psychosom Med. 2010;72(3):324–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Moore SR, Depue RA. Neurobehavioral foundation of environmental reactivity. Psychol Bull. 2016;142(2):107–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Bromberg-Martin ES, Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron. 2010;68(5):815–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Wise RA. Role of brain dopamine in food reward and reinforcement. Philos Trans R Soc Biol Sci. 2006;361(1471):1149–58.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: implications for obesity. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(1):37–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Volkow ND, Wise RA, Baler R. The dopamine motive system: implications for drug and food addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18(12):741.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gilsbach S, Neufang S, Scherag S, Vloet TD, Fink GR, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, et al. Effects of the DRD4 genotype on neural networks associated with executive functions in children and adolescents. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2(4):417–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Silveira PP, Portella AK, Kennedy JL, Gaudreau H, Davis C, Steiner M, et al. Association between the seven-repeat allele of the dopamine-4 receptor gene (DRD4) and spontaneous food intake in pre-school children. Appetite. 2014;73:15–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwaldt S, Paterson A, Jovanovic V, Van Tol HH. Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J Neurochem. 1995;65(3):1157–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Van Tol HH, Wu CM, Guan HC, Ohara K, Bunzow JR, Civelli O, et al. Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature. 1992;358(6382):149–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. D’Souza UM, Russ C, Tahir E, Mill J, McGuffin P, Asherson PJ, et al. Functional effects of a tandem duplication polymorphism in the 5′ flanking region of the DRD4 gene. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(9):691–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Silveira PPPI, Gaudreau H, Silveira PP, Pokhvisneva I, Gaudreau H, Atkinson L, Fleming AS, Sokolowski MB, Steiner M, Kennedy JL, Dubé L, Levitan RD, Meaney MJ, MAVAN research teamAtkinson L, Fleming AS, Sokolowski MB, Steiner M, Kennedy JL, Dubé L, Levitan RD, Meaney MJ, MAVAN research team. Fetal growth interacts with multilocus genetic score reflecting dopamine signaling capacity to predict spontaneous sugar intake in children. Appetite. 2018;120(Jan):596–601.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Dalle Molle R, Fatemi H, Dagher A, Levitan RD, Silveira PP, Dube L. Gene and environment interaction: is the differential susceptibility hypothesis relevant for obesity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;73:326–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(6):885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17(2):271–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Babineau V, Green CG, Jolicoeur-Martineau A, Bouvette-Turcot AA, Minde K, Sassi R, et al. Prenatal depression and 5-HTTLPR interact to predict dysregulation from 3 to 36 months–a differential susceptibility model. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(1):21–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Taylor SE, Way BM, Welch WT, Hilmert CJ, Lehman BJ, Eisenberger NI. Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(7):671–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kitayama S, King A, Hsu M, Liberzon I, Yoon C. Dopamine-system genes and cultural acquisition: the norm sensitivity hypothesis. Curr Opin Psychol. 2016;8:167–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Kitayama S, King A, Yoon C, Tompson S, Huff S, Liberzon I. The dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) moderates cultural difference in independent versus interdependent social orientation. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(6):1169–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Silveira PP, Gaudreau H, Atkinson L, Fleming AS, Sokolowski MB, Steiner M, et al. Genetic differential susceptibility to socioeconomic status and childhood obesogenic behavior: why targeted prevention may be the best societal investment. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(4):359–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Barth B, Bizarro L, Miguel PM, Dubé L, Levitan R, O'Donnell K, et al. Genetically predicted gene expression of prefrontal DRD4 gene and the differential susceptibility to childhood emotional eating in response to positive environment. Appetite. 2020;148:104594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Black C, Moon G, Baird J. Dietary inequalities: what is the evidence for the effect of the neighbourhood food environment? Health Place. 2014;27:229–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Labonte ME, Cyr A, Baril-Gravel L, Royer MM, Lamarche B. Validity and reproducibility of a web-based, self-administered food frequency questionnaire. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2012;66(2):166–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Le Moullec N, Deheeger M, Preziosi P, Monteiro P, Valeix P, Rolland-Cachera M-F, et al. Validation du manuel-photos utilisé pour l’enquête alimentaire de l’étude SU.VI.MAX. Cahiers de nutrition et de diététique. 1996;31(3):158–64.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Schakel SF, Sievert YA, Buzzard IM. Sources of data for developing and maintaining a nutrient database. J Am Diet Assoc. 1988;88(10):1268–71.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Verdier P, Beare-Rogers J. The Canadian nutrient file. J Can Dietetic Assoc. 1984;45(1):52–5.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Health Canada, Canadian Nutrient File (CNF). [Available from: www.healthcanada.gc.ca/cnf].

  42. O'Donnell KA, Gaudreau H, Colalillo S, Steiner M, Atkinson L, Moss E, et al. The maternal adversity, vulnerability and neurodevelopment project: theory and methodology. Can J Psychiatry. 2014;59(9):497–508.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Ordre Professional des Diététistes du Québec. Manuel de nutrition clinique. 3rd ed. Quebec: Ordre Professional des Diététistes du Québec; 2000.

  44. Katamay SW, Esslinger KA, Vigneault M, Johnston JL, Junkins BA, Robbins LG, et al. Eating well with Canada's food guide (2007): development of the food intake pattern. Nutr Rev. 2007;65(4):155–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Drewnowski A. Energy density, palatability, and satiety: implications for weight control. Nutr Rev. 1998;56(12):347–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Blechert J, Klackl J, Miedl SF, Wilhelm FH. To eat or not to eat: effects of food availability on reward system activity during food picture viewing. Appetite. 2016;99:254–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Lichter JB, Barr CL, Kennedy JL, Van Tol HH, Kidd KK, Livak KJ. A hypervariable segment in the human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene. Hum Mol Genet. 1993;2(6):767–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Children’s food environment state indicator report 2011. Atlanta (GA): CDC; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Statistics Canada. Loc income cut-offs for 2005 and low income measures for 2004. 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Roisman GI, Newman DA, Fraley RC, Haltigan JD, Groh AM, Haydon KC. Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(2):389–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Widaman KF, Helm JL, Castro-Schilo L, Pluess M, Stallings MC, Belsky J. Distinguishing ordinal and disordinal interactions. Psychol Methods. 2012;17(4):615.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Fraley RC. Probing Interactions in Differential Susceptibility Research: University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; 2019 [01/12/2019]. Available from: https://www.yourpersonality.net/interaction/.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Herring MP, Sailors MH, Bray MS. Genetic factors in exercise adoption, adherence and obesity. Obes Rev. 2014;15(1):29–39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Wilson G, Wood K. The influence of children on parental purchases during supermarket shopping. Int J Consum Stud. 2004;28(4):329–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Campbell S, James EL, Stacey FG, Bowman J, Chapman K, Kelly B. A mixed-method examination of food marketing directed towards children in Australian supermarkets. Health Promot Int. 2014;29(2):267–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kelly B, Chapman K, Hardy LL, King L, Farrell L. Parental awareness and attitudes of food marketing to children: a community attitudes survey of parents in New South Wales, Australia. J Paediatr Child Health. 2009;45(9):493–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Bauer KW, Weeks HM, Lumeng JC, Miller AL, Gearhardt AN. Maternal executive function and the family food environment. Appetite. 2019;137:21–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The voluntary participation of all participants is greatly appreciated. We thank the MAVAN study group and all staff. The original MAVAN study was funded by a grant from the Canadian Institute of Health and Research to M. Meaney. Additional funding was granted by the Norlien Foundation (Calgary, Canada), the WOCO Foundation (London, Canada), the Blema & Arnold Steinberg Family Foundation, the Faculty of Medicine of McGill University, and Jacobs Foundation (Switzerland).

Funding

The BtS study was funded by a Collaborative Team Grant, Canada – India Initiative on Childhood funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Indian Council of Medical Research (85512). The original MAVAN study was funded by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health and Research to support the establishment of M.A.V.A.N. (Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability and Human Development) project (191827) as well as several other grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Additional funding was provided by the National Institutes of Health (5 U01 HD061973–03). The funding bodies were not involved in any aspect of the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CP: Writing original draft, conceptualisation, methodology, analyses, interpretation, review and editing. AK: Write up, interpretation, conceptualisation, review and editing. SM: Conceptualisation, interpretation, review and editing. YM: Conceptualisation, methodology, review and editing. AD: Conceptualisation, interpretation, review and editing. JK: Methodology, grant support, review and editing. RL: Conceptualisation, interpretation, methodology, review and editing. MM: Conceptualisation, funding, review and editing. PS: Conceptualisation, interpretation, methodology, review and editing. LD: Conceptualisation, writing, funding, interpretation, review and editing. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Catherine Paquet.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Approval for the MAVAN project was obtained from obstetricians who performed deliveries at the study hospitals and by the ethics committees and university affiliates (McGill University and Université de Montréal, the Royal Victoria Hospital, Jewish General Hospital, Centre hospitalier de l’Université de Montréal, and Hôpital Maisonneuve-Rosemount) and St Joseph Hospital and McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Verbal consent was obtained from parents or guardians for all BtS participants at the time of the first data collection and written consent for follow up assessments. Verbal consent was selected given the method used for the first data collection (phone recruitment followed by interview). Ethical approval for all aspects of the BtS study (including verbal consent) was obtained from McGill University’s Institutional Research Board. In both studies, parents or legal guardians provided consent for their children.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Differential Susceptibility testing – Overview. This additional file provides additional details on the differential susceptibility testing measures used in the manuscript and their interpretation.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Paquet, C., Portella, A.K., Moore, S. et al. Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates real-world behavioural response to the food retail environment in children. BMC Public Health 21, 145 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-021-10160-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-021-10160-w

Keywords