Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Examining social-cognitive theory constructs as mediators of behaviour change in the active team smartphone physical activity program: a mediation analysis

Abstract

Background

Regular engagement in physical activity has well-established physical and psychological health benefits. Despite this, over a quarter of the global adult population is insufficiently physically active. Physical activity interventions grounded in behaviour change theory, such as the social-cognitive theory, are widely considered to be more effective than non-theoretical approaches. Such interventions set out to intervene on the ultimate outcome (physical activity), but also influence intermediate factors (social-cognitive theory constructs) which in turn, are believed to influence physical activity behaviour. The primary aim of the study was to use mediation analysis to examine whether changes in the social-cognitive theory and related constructs, in particular self-efficacy, outcome expectations, intentions, barriers and goal setting, mediated the effects of a smartphone-based social networking physical activity intervention.

Methods

Mediation analyses were conducted using the PROCESS Macro in SPSS to (i) calculate the regression coefficients for the effect of the independent variable (group allocation) on the hypothesised mediators (social-cognitive theory constructs), (ii) calculate the regression coefficient for the effect of the hypothesised mediators (social-cognitive theory constructs) on the dependent variable (objectively measured physical activity or self-report physical activity), independent of group assignment and (iii) determine the total, direct and indirect intervention effects.

Results

Data from 243 participants were included in the mediation analysis. There was no evidence of mediation for change in objectively measured MVPA or self-reported MVPA.

Conclusions

There was no conclusive evidence that any of the social-cognitive theory constructs mediated the relationship between an app-based intervention and change in physical activity. Ongoing efforts to develop and understand components that make physical activity app-based interventions effective are recommended.

Trial registration

This trial was registered with the Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trial Registry (ACTRN12617000113358, date of registration 23 January, 2017).

Peer Review reports

Background

Physical activity has well established psychological and physical health benefits. Most notably, engagement in regular physical activity is linked to improved muscular and cardiorespiratory fitness, functional health and mental health [1,2,3], as well as the prevention of primary and secondary chronic disease and decreasing all causes of mortality [4]. Despite this, over a quarter (27.5%) of the global adult population is insufficiently physically active [5]. At present, global recommendations on physical activity for health recommend adults aged between 18 and 64 years partake in at least 75 min of vigorous-intensity physical activity, or 150 min of moderate-intensity physical activity, per week [6].

Smartphones are a prominent feature in modern daily life and are developing as a promising platform for delivering public health programs. In 2017, approximately two thirds of the global adult population owned a smartphone [7] with more time being spent on a smartphone than any other device [8]. The high usage, convenience and appeal of smartphone platforms make them an attractive tool for physical activity interventions [9].

Physical activity interventions grounded in behaviour change theory, such as the social-cognitive theory, are widely considered to be more effective than non-theoretical approaches [10,11,12]. Such interventions typically set out to intervene on the ultimate outcome (physical activity), but also influence intermediate constructs which, in turn, are believed to influence physical activity behaviour [13]. The social-cognitive theory asserts that positively impacting the intermediate constructs of self-efficacy (the belief that an individual can effectively control their health habits) [14], outcome expectations (the expected benefits and efforts of adjusting health behaviours) [14], intentions to engage in physical activity, perceived barriers of engagement in physical activity and the setting of physical activity goals, is essential to underpin change in the target health behaviour. Basing a physical activity program on the social-cognitive theory is typically done through the inclusion of mechanisms such as social interaction and support, goal setting, feedback in real-time, rewards and incentives. In order to refine and improve interventions, it is essential to explore the effectiveness of particular components and mechanisms which produce behavioural change [15,16,17].

One way to explore how the mechanisms of behavioural change may influence an intended outcome is through conducting a mediation analysis. A mediation analysis is a statistical approach that seeks to identify and explain factors that underlie an observed relationship between an independent variable and a dependent variable, by including a third variable known as a mediator [18, 19] (See Fig. 1 in Methods). In mediation analyses, it is assumed that mediation has occurred when the indirect effect (Path AB) is significant [20].

A handful of previous studies have examined whether the social-cognitive theory mediates the physical activity intervention’s effectiveness, with mixed findings [21]. Some studies have reported self-efficacy [22,23,24] and goal setting [23, 25] to have significant mediating effects on physical activity. However, others have failed to find evidence of mediation [26, 27]. The extent to which theory is embedded within intervention design varies [28], as does the mechanisms used [29]. There is some evidence to suggest that the effectiveness of theory based interventions may vary in certain subgroups, for example, based on age or sex [21]. This has been attributed to certain subgroups showing a higher degree of intrinsic motivation towards behaviour change [21].

The social-cognitive theory and related constructs have also been used to inform interventions targeting other health behaviours (i.e. weight loss or nutrition) where physical activity is a secondary outcome. Similarly, results on the effectiveness of these constructs mediating physical activity as a secondary outcome are varied [30].

Whilst a vast number of smartphone physical activity applications exist, and many contain theory-based features [31], few have been rigorously evaluated [32] and even fewer have undergone detailed analyses to understand the behaviour change mechanisms that are contributing to effective or ineffective outcomes [33]. To our knowledge, no studies to date have examined potential mediators of behaviour change in a physical activity smartphone intervention.

This study aimed to address this gap. The primary aim of this study was to examine whether changes in the social-cognitive theory and related constructs, in particular self-efficacy, outcome expectations, intentions, barriers and goal setting, mediated the effect of a smartphone-based social networking physical activity intervention. The secondary aim was to undertake hypothesis-generating analyses to determine if mediation was present within subgroups of participants based on age.

Methods

Ethics approval was provided by the University of South Australia Human Research Ethics Committee, protocol number 0000033967. All participants provided informed consent prior to participation.

Study design and sample size

This study is a secondary analysis using data from a large-scale randomised controlled trial (RCT) evaluating the effects of an online social networking physical activity intervention delivered via smartphone app, “Active Team”. Full details of the RCT are provided elsewhere [34]. Briefly, four-hundred and forty-four (444) participants were recruited between October 2016 to December 2017. Participants were aged 18–65 years old, Australian residents, fluent in English, self-reported attaining less than 150 min of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) per week at time of enrolment, and used Facebook. Participants enrolled in the study in clusters of three to eight Facebook friends, the first participant joining the intervention was designated as the team “captain” and their friends as their “team members”. Participant clusters were randomly allocated to one of three groups: a socially-enhanced intervention group, basic intervention group and waitlist control group. This mediation analysis is based on data from the socially-enhanced intervention group (n= 141) and the waitlist control group (n= 143). The basic intervention group was excluded from this analysis due to its features not being based on the social-cognitive theory.

Intervention details

Participant clusters allocated to the socially-enhanced intervention group received a pedometer, access to the Active Team app and were challenged with the goal of taking 10,000 steps a day for 100 days. The Active Team app features were developing based on the social-cognitive theory [35], and incorporates individuals’ pre-existing social networks by linking to Facebook and encouraging social interaction and enjoyment through social and gamified features such as ability to send virtual gifts, compete in mini challenges, view progress on a leader board, unlock features, and post messages and photographs to the Facebook-style newsfeed. Participant clusters allocated to the waitlist control group were instructed to go about their usual daily activities and received access to the Active Team program at the end of the study. Full details of the study’s main findings are published elsewhere [36]. Briefly, from baseline to post-intervention (3-month assessment), objectively measured MVPA increased by an average of 11 (SD 329) minutes/week in the socially-enhanced intervention group and increased by an average of 3 (SD 316) minutes/week in the waitlist control group (non-significant difference between groups). Over the same period, self-reported MVPA increased by an average of 181 (SD 316) minutes/week in the socially-enhanced intervention group, compared to an average increase of 93 (SD 288) minutes/week in the waitlist control group. Subgroup analyses suggested that intervention effectiveness was associated with age (p=0.002) but not sex, BMI, or education.

Outcome measures

Outcome measures were assessed at baseline, end of program (i.e., 3 months post baseline) and 6 months post program completion (i.e., 9 months post baseline). Change in MVPA from baseline to 3 months for objective and self-reported physical activity has been used for this analysis.

Physical activity (objective)

Objectively measured MVPA was collected using wrist-worn GENEActiv accelerometers for 7-days at each assessment time point. The GENEActiv accelerometers output raw data regarding the frequency, duration and intensity of physical activity [37]. They are highly reliable, with intra-instrument and inter-instrument coefficients of variation of 1.8 and 2.4% respectively [38]. Similarly, when compared in a mechanical shaker, GENEActiv accelerometers have excellent validity (r=0.89) [38]. Minimum wear criteria were used to determine valid accelerometer data for inclusion in data analyses: participants must have worn the accelerometer for at least 10 waking hours, on four or more days, including one weekend day. Participants returning incomplete data were asked to wear the GENEActiv accelerometer again up to two more times [36]. Accelerometer files were processed with 60 s epochs and the Esliger cutpoints [38] were used to define MVPA as any activity above 645 counts per minute. Daily average (weighted as 5X [weekday average] + 2X [weekend day average]/7) was multiplied by seven to calculate mean weekly minutes of objective MVPA.

Physical activity (self-reported)

Self-reported MVPA data were collected using the 8-item Active Australia Survey (AAS). The AAS asks participants to recall the frequency, duration, intensity and type of physical activity performed within the previous week [39]. For example, “In the last week, how many times have you walked continuously for at least 10 minutes, for recreation, exercise or to get to or from places?” and “What do you estimate was the total time you spent walking in this way in the last week?”. The AAS has acceptable repeatability when compared with three other self-reported physical activity measures (k= 0.52 95%CI 0.44–0.60) [40] and validity compared with accelerometery data (rs = 0.61 95%CI 0.43–0.75) [41].

Social cognitive theory measures

The constructs self-efficacy, outcome expectations, intentions, perceived barriers, and goals in relation to participating in regular physical activity were assessed at each time point via 21-items.

Self-efficacy for physical activity under varying circumstances (i.e., tired, in a bad mood, do it alone, when it becomes boring, can’t notice fitness improvements, competing demands on time, feel stiff or sore, bad weather) was assessed via 8 items (Cronbach’s α = 0.90) [42] and responses were on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “not at all confident” to “extremely confident” [42].

Outcome expectations related to potential positive benefits of physical activity (i.e., reduce tension or manage stress, confidence about health, better sleep, positive outlook, control weight) were assessed via 5 items (Cronbach’s α = 0.83) [42]. Participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” [42].

Intentions were assessed via 2 items (i.e., motivated, determined) to engage in regular physical activity (Cronbach’s α = 0.91) [43]. Participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “not at all motivated/determined” to “extremely motivated/determined” [43].

Perceived barriers to participating in physical activity were assessed using 5 items (i.e., take too much time, less time with friends and family, too many other responsibilities, worry about looking awkward, would cost too much money, Cronbach’s α = 0.72) [42], responses were on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” [42].

Goals relative to physical activity was assessed with a single-item (i.e., “I often set physical activity goals”) with a 5-point Likert scale response option ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” [44].

Sociodemographic variables (e.g., age, sex, education) were also collected at baseline. These variables were self-reported and included age, sex (male or female), education level (high school or less; technical or further education; university degree or higher), height and weight from which body mass index (BMI) was calculated and marital status (single, partner, prefer not to say).

Statistical analyses

Change in MVPA from baseline to 3 months was calculated for objective and self-reported physical activity. Outliers were detected on the basis of change scores falling outside of 3.0 standard deviations from the mean [45], resulting in three outliers being removed from the self-reported MVPA data (lower limit − 843 min/week, upper limit 1249 min/week).

Simple mediation models were conducted with the intervention allocation (either control or socially-enhanced group) as the independent variable. Mediator variables were mean difference from baseline to 3 months in the social-cognitive theory constructs of self-efficacy, outcome expectations, intentions, barriers and goals, with each construct tested individually. The dependent variables were mean differences from baseline to 3 months in objective and self-reported MVPA. Mediation analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 23 using the PROCESS INDIRECT Macro (Model 4) [19] to (i) calculate the unstandardized regression coefficients for the effect of the independent variable (intervention allocation) on the hypothesised mediators (social-cognitive theory construct change scores) (Path A), (ii) calculate the unstandardized regression coefficient for the effect of the hypothesised mediators (social-cognitive theory construct change scores) on the dependent variable (physical activity change scores), independent of intervention allocation (Path B) and (iii) determine the total (Path C), direct (Path C′) and indirect (Path AB) intervention effects (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Simple and Mediated Relationship Model

The PROCESS macro automatically handles missing data through listwise deletion, which was considered appropriate as Little’s test accepted the null-hypothesis that the data were missing completely at random (X2= 4.877 df = 7, P = .675). As such, only participants with complete data were included in the mediation analysis (243 participants when the dependent variable was self-reported MVPA and 222 participants when the dependent variable was objectively measured MVPA).

The PROCESS macro generates bias-corrected bootstrapped 95% asymmetrical confidence intervals around the indirect effect. For mediation to be present, the indirect effect (Path AB) must be significant [19, 20]. Using Rucker and colleagues’ [20] recommendations for mediation analyses, mediation will be explored even in the absence of a significant total (Path C) or direct (Path C′) effect.

Given that intervention effectiveness was associated with age, but not other sociodemographic variables (sex, BMI, education category) (see Additional File 1), a subgroup analysis was conducted to determine whether social-cognitive theory variables mediated intervention effectiveness in older and younger participants. Participants were dichotomised based on being ≤40 years of age and > 40 years of age. Treating age in this manner meant that if findings were significant (i.e. that social-cognitive theory variables mediated intervention effects in one age group but not the other) their relevance for future research would be clear to interpret (i.e. suggesting that future social-cognitive theory physical activity interventions may be more fruitful if they were targeted at older or younger adults). For all analyses, an alpha < 0.05 was used to denote statistical significance, with Holm’s Sequential Bonferroni adjustment applied to reduce the risk of Type 1 errors (false positives) due to multiple comparisons. All analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 23.

Results

Table 1 presents baseline characteristics of the study participants. The mean age of participants was 41.5 (SD 11.3) years, the majority (74.8%) were female. The mean BMI of participants was 30.0 (SD 6.8) which is categorised as on the border between overweight and obese [46]. Baseline, 3-month and mean change values for the mediator variables and dependent variables are detailed in Table 2.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of study participants
Table 2 Baseline, 3-month and mean change in mediator and dependent variables for intervention and control groups

Mediation analysis

Data for all pathways within the simple and mediated relationship model has been reported in Table 3 and Table 4. All reported values in text and in tables are unstandardized regression coefficients, adjusted for baseline values. The indirect effect of the intervention on objective MVPA and self-reported MVPA data was examined to determine the presence of mediation. There was no statistically significant indirect effect when any of the potentially mediating variables; self-efficacy, intentions, outcome expectations, perceived barriers and goals were included in the model for objective MVPA (see Table 3). Similarly, there was no statistically significant indirect effect when any of the social-cognitive theory constructs were examined as mediators of self-reported MVPA (see Table 4). As such, the criteria for mediation were not satisfied.

Table 3 Results of the mediation model for objectively measured MVPA. N = 222
Table 4 Results of the mediation model for self-reported MVPA. N=243

Subgroup analysis

A mediation analysis was undertaken on the basis of age subgroups (younger adults [18–40 years; Table 5] and older adults [> 40 years; Table 6]). Amongst participants aged 18–40 years, no statistically significant indirect effect was identified when any of the social-cognitive theory constructs were the mediator variable. Thus, mediation was not present for any of the social-cognitive theory constructs within this sub-group. Similarly, there was no statistically significant indirect effect when any of the social-cognitive theory constructs were the mediator variable in the sub-group of participants aged 41 years or older, again, indicating no mediation.

Table 5 Results of the mediation model for participants aged 18–40 years. N=124
Table 6 Results of the mediation model for participants aged 41 years or older. N=122

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to determine if the social-cognitive theory and related constructs of self-efficacy, outcome expectations, intentions, perceived barriers and goals; mediated change in physical activity for participants of an app-based physical activity intervention. For both objective MVPA and self-reported MVPA, the indirect effects were not significant and as such there was no evidence of mediation. Furthermore, there was no conclusive evidence of mediation within subgroups of participants based on age. Thus, in all, this study failed to find evidence of mediation for any of the social-cognitive theory constructs.

The lack of evidence supporting the notion that social-cognitive theory constructs mediate the effects of a physical activity intervention is consistent with previous studies which similarly reported no evidence of mediation [26, 27, 47]. Only a small change from baseline to 3-months was recorded in all social-cognitive theory constructs within this study. Notably, this change was in a negative (i.e. undesirable) direction for the constructs of self-efficacy, outcome expectations and intentions. Whilst on the face of it, this result is surprising, it is actually consistent with results from previous mediation analyses of physical activity interventions that have also reported a negative direction of change for self-efficacy [48, 49] and outcome expectancy [48] from baseline to follow-up. One explanation for the negative direction of change may be initial elevation bias: participants may have overestimated their baseline function for the social-cognitive theory constructs, thus leaving minimal room for improvement [50]. This is particularly true for the constructs of outcome expectations and intentions to engage in physical activity, where mean baseline data for the intervention group was 4.32 (SD 0.57) and 4.09 (SD 0.59) out of a maximum five respectively. Initial elevation bias is common in self-reported data [50], and particularly self-reported data of internal states [50] as is the social-cognitive theory constructs.

Alternatively, the negative direction of change for the constructs of self-efficacy, outcome expectations and intentions may be explained by the response-shift theory. The response-shift theory suggests changes in a measured variable (e.g. physical activity) may lead to change in an individual’s self-evaluation [51]. For example, prior to engaging in the Active Team smartphone physical activity program, participants may have had optimistic expectations toward changing physical activity behaviour. However, once engaging in the program it is possible participants’ experiences of barriers and constraints prompted a realisation that changing physical activity behaviour is more difficult than initially perceived. Whilst the response-shift theory is yet to be explored in depth within the social-cognitive domain [52], it may provide an explanation for the small and negative direction of change in the social-cognitive theory constructs.

This study’s finding that the social-cognitive theory constructs did not appear to mediate the relationship between the intervention and change in physical activity adds to the ongoing debate about the role of theory in behaviour change programs. Conventionally, it is widely accepted that theory-based physical activity interventions are more effective than non-theoretical approaches [10,11,12]. However, two recent meta-analyses have questioned this, finding that interventions based on the social-cognitive theory were no more effective than non-theoretical based interventions [28, 29]. The contrasting findings suggest that the role and implementation of theory in intervention design is not well understood.

Rather than theory having no role, it is possible our intervention, and others that have failed to demonstrate mediation, may not have operationalised social-cognitive theory effectively. One systematic review reported that interventions which extensively incorporated theory into their design had larger effect sizes than those with less or no use of theory [29]. The Active Team intervention design was informed by the social-cognitive theory; however, design emphasis was on usability and enjoyability. This is in contrast to previous physical activity interventions which emphasised the social-cognitive theory through the incorporation of educational modules [22, 23, 25]. This study’s finding of no statistically-significant mediating effects of social-cognitive theory constructs on physical activity, may be a consequence of the social-cognitive theory solely guiding the development of the smartphone app intervention and not being a prominent feature of the intervention, as reported in previous studies [29, 53].

In light of these findings, the social-cognitive theory could be more wholly embedded within the Active Team intervention in a number of ways; personalised step goals, adjusted according to user performance and preference to enhance self-efficacy; use of notifications/reminders to improve users’ outcome expectations regarding physical activity (i.e. improved vitality [54], functional health [1], mental wellbeing [55] and social opportunity [56]); inclusion of season-based app features, goals and social challenges (e.g. indoor challenges during winter when the weather is likely to be poor and outdoor challenges during warmer weather months) to address perceived barriers. These additional features aligned with the social-cognitive theory components may produce a more significant change in the mediator variables [29].

A strength of this study is that it is the first to attempt to examine the mediating effect of the social-cognitive theory constructs in the context of an app-based physical activity intervention. Additional strengths include the study’s large sample size, use of established social-cognitive theory measurement tools and high-quality outcome measures, including accelerometry.

It is important to acknowledge limitations, including the use of self-reported physical activity measures, which are susceptible to social desirability bias, response bias, initial elevation bias and recall bias [57]. In addition, our analysis approach focused on change scores, which can be susceptible to measurement error and does not account for baseline values [58]. Furthermore, this mediation analysis was conducted as a secondary analysis, rather than being an experiment designed with a primary focus on understanding intervention mechanisms [59]. The PROCESS Macro in SPSS does not account for clustered data, however since this mediation analysis did not find evidence of mediation, accounting for clustering will not change the results. Additionally, the social-cognitive theory tools used in this study lack comprehensive evidence of reliability, validity and sensitivity to change. They were selected after extensive literature searching which failed to identify alternatives with established psychometric properties, thus the current tools were selected on the basis that they had been used in previous research. Given these limitations, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution.

The use of smartphone apps as a platform for delivering physical activity interventions is in its infancy. This study is the first mediation analysis of an app-based physical activity intervention. As such it is important to report all findings, including those that may be non-supportive, as they can contribute to understanding of imperative and unnecessary intervention components, to then facilitate the development of more effective intervention designs [60,61,62].

Conclusion

There was no conclusive evidence that any of the social-cognitive theory constructs of self-efficacy, outcome expectations, intentions, perceived barriers or goals mediated the relationship between an app-based intervention and change in physical activity. Ongoing efforts to develop and understand components that make physical activity app-based interventions effective are recommended.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

AAS:

Active Australia Survey

BMI:

Body mass index

MVPA:

Moderate to vigorous physical activity

RCT:

Randomised controlled trial

References

  1. World Health Organisation. Physical activity: World Health Organisation. 2018. Available from: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs385/en/. Archived at: http://www.webcitation.org/71qo8nOXd.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Warburton DER, Nicol CW, Bredin SSD. Health benefits of physical activity: the evidence. Can Med Assoc J. 2006;174(6):801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Reiner M, Niermann C, Jekauc D, Woll A. Long-term health benefits of physical activity – a systematic review of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):813.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Kruk J. Physical activity in the prevention of the most frequent chronic diseases: an analysis of the recent evidence. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2007;8(3):325–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Worldwide trends in insufficient physical activity from 2001 to 2016: a pooled analysis of 358 population-based surveys with 1.9 million participants. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6(10):e1077–e86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. World Health Organisation. Global recommendations on physical activity for health. Geneva: World Health Organisation; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Molla R. Two-thirds of adults worldwide will own a smartphones next year: recode. 2017. Available from: https://www.recode.net/2017/10/16/16482168/two-thirds-of-adults-worldwide-will-own-smartphones-next-year. Archived at: http://www.webcitation.org/71qofnqQe.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Lu T. Almost half of smartphone users spend more than 5 hours a day on their mobile device: Cunterpoint Technology Market Research. 2017. Available from: https://www.counterpointresearch.com/almost-half-of-smartphone-users-spend-more-than-5-hours-a-day-on-their-mobile-device/.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Maher C, Ryan J, Kernot J, Podsiadly J, Keenihan S. Social media and applications to health behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. 2016;9:50–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Stacey FG, James EL, Chapman K, Courneya KS, Lubans DR. A systematic review and meta-analysis of social cognitive theory-based physical activity and/or nutrition behavior change interventions for cancer survivors. J Cancer Surviv. 2015;9(2):305–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Plotnikoff RC, Lubans DR, Penfold CM, Courneya KS. Testing mediator variables in a physical activity intervention for women with type 2 diabetes. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2014;15(1):1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Michie S, Abraham C. Interventions to change health behaviours: evidence-based or evidence-inspired? Psychol Health. 2004;19(1):29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Lewis BA, Marcus BH, Pate RR, Dunn AL. Psychosocial mediators of physical activity behavior among adults and children. Am J Prev Med. 2002;23(2 Suppl):26–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bandura A. Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ Behav. 2004;31(2):143–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Bauman AE, Sallis JF, Dzewaltowski DA, Owen N. Toward a better understanding of the influences on physical activity: the role of determinants, correlates, causal variables, mediators, moderators, and confounders. Am J Prev Med. 2002;23(2 Suppl):5–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kraemer HC, Wilson GT, Fairburn CG, Agras WS. Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(10):877–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Moore GF, Audrey S, Barker M, Bond L, Bonell C, Hardeman W, et al. Process evaluation of complex interventions: Medical Research Council guidance. BMJ (Clin Res Ed). 2015;350:h1258.

    Google Scholar 

  18. MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Fritz MS. Mediation analysis. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:593.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation and conditional Process analysis: a Regressin-based approach. 1st ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Rucker DD, Preacher KJ, Tormala ZL, Petty RE. Mediation analysis in social psychology: current practices and new recommendations. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2011;5(6):359–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Young MD, Plotnikoff RC, Collins CE, Callister R, Morgan PJ. Social cognitive theory and physical activity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2014;15(12):983–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Anderson ES, Winett RA, Wojcik JR, Williams DM. Social cognitive mediators of change in a group randomized nutrition and physical activity intervention: social support, self-efficacy, outcome expectations and self-regulation in the guide-to-health trial. J Health Psychol. 2010;15(1):21–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Taymoori P, Lubans DR. Mediators of behavior change in two tailored physical activity interventions for adolescent girls. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2008;9(5):605–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Dishman RK, Motl RW, Saunders R, Felton G, Ward DS, Dowda M, et al. Self-efficacy partially mediates the effect of a school-based physical-activity intervention among adolescent girls. Prev Med. 2004;38(5):628–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Stacey FG, James EL, Chapman K, Lubans DR. Social cognitive theory mediators of physical activity in a lifestyle program for cancer survivors and carers: findings from the ENRICH randomized controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13:49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Callister R. Potential moderators and mediators of intervention effects in an obesity prevention program for adolescent boys from disadvantaged schools. J Sci Med Sport. 2012;15(6):519–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Lubans DR, Sylva K. Mediators of change following a senior school physical activity intervention. J Sci Med Sport. 2009;12(1):134–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Prestwich A, Sniehotta F, Whittington C, Dombrowski S, Rogers L, Michie S. Does theory influence the effectiveness of health behavior interventions? Meta-Analysis. Health Psychol. 2014;33(5):465–74 ISSN 0278-6133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Webb TL, Joseph J, Yardley L, Michie S. Using the internet to promote health behavior change: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the impact of theoretical basis, use of behavior change techniques, and mode of delivery on efficacy. J Med Internet Res. 2010;12(1):e4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Teixeira PJ, Carraça EV, Marques MM, Rutter H, Oppert J-M, De Bourdeaudhuij I, et al. Successful behavior change in obesity interventions in adults: a systematic review of self-regulation mediators. BMC Med. 2015;13(1):84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Direito A, Dale LP, Shields E, Dobson R, Whittaker R, Maddison R. Do physical activity and dietary smartphone applications incorporate evidence-based behaviour change techniques? BMC Public Health. 2014;14:646.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Romeo A, Edney S, Plotnikoff R, Curtis R, Ryan J, Sanders I, et al. Can smartphone apps increase physical activity? Systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res. 2019;21(3):e12053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Nurmi J, Knittle K, Helf C, Zwickl P, Lusilla Palacios P, Castellano Tejedor C, et al. A personalised, sensor-based smart phone intervention for physical activity and diet ? PRECIOUS N-of-1 trial. Front Public Health. 2016.

  34. Edney S, Plotnikoff R, Vandelanotte C, Olds T, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Ryan J, et al. “Active Team” a social and gamified app-based physical activity intervention: randomised controlled trial study protocol. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(1):859.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Bandura A. Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol Health. 1998;13(4):623–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Edney SM, Olds TS, Ryan JC, Vandelanotte C, Plotnikoff RC, Curtis RG, et al. A social networking and gamified app to increase physical activity: cluster RCT. Am J Prev Med. 2020;58(2):e51–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Activinsights. Wrist-worn accelerometer research watches cambridgeshire: Activinsights; 2017. [cited 2017 10 September]. Available from: https://www.activinsights.com/products/geneactiv/.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Esliger D, Rowlands A, Hurst T, Catt M, Murray P, Eston R. Validation of the GENEA accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43(6):1085–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. The Active Australia Survey: a guide and manual for implementation, analysis and reporting Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare; 2003. Available from: https://www.aihw.gov.au/getmedia/ff25c134-5df2-45ba-b4e1-6c214ed157e6/aas.pdf.aspx?inline=true.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Brown WJ, Trost SG, Bauman A, Mummery K, Owen N. Test-retest reliability of four physical activity measures used in population surveys. J Sci Med Sport. 2004;7(2):205–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Fjeldsoe BS, Winkler EAH, Marshall AL, Eakin EG, Reeves MM. Active adults recall their physical activity differently to less active adults: test–retest reliability and validity of a physical activity survey. Health Promot J Austr. 2013;24(1):26–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Plotnikoff RC, Blanchard C, Hotz SB, Rhodes R. Validation of the decisional balance scales in the exercise domain from the transtheoretical model: a longitudinal test. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2001;5(4):191–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Rhodes RE, Hunt Matheson D, Mark R. Evaluation of social cognitive scaling response options in the physical activity domain. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2010;14(3):137–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Rovniak LS, Anderson ES, Winett RA, Stephens RS. Social cognitive determinants of physical activity in young adults: a prospective structural equation analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2002;24(2):149–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 4th ed. Needham: Allyn & Bacon; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  46. WHO. Obesity and Overweight: World Health Organisation. 2018. [cited 2018 29 August]. Available from: http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Fjeldsoe BS, Miller YD, Marshall AL. Social cognitive mediators of the effect of the MobileMums intervention on physical activity. Health Psychol. 2013;32(7):729–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Hortz B, Petosa RL. Social cognitive theory variables mediation of moderate exercise. Am J Health Behav. 2008;32(3):305–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Haerens L, Cerin E, Maes L, Cardon G, Deforche B, De Bourdeaudhuij I. Explaining the effect of a 1-year intervention promoting physical activity in middle schools: a mediation analysis. Public Health Nutr. 2008;11(5):501–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Shrout PE, Stadler G, Lane SP, McClure MJ, Jackson GL, Clavél FD, et al. Initial elevation bias in subjective reports. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(1):E15–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Sprangers MAG, Schwartz CE. Integrating response shift into health-related quality of life research: a theoretical model. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(11):1507–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Vallance JKH, Courneya KS, Plotnikoff RC, Mackey JR. Analyzing theoretical mechanisms of physical activity behavior change in breast cancer survivors: results from the activity promotion (ACTION) trial. Ann Behav Med. 2008;35(2):150–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Bluethmann SM, Bartholomew LK, Murphy CC, Vernon SW. Use of theory in behavior change interventions: an analysis of programs to increase physical activity in Posttreatment breast cancer survivors. Health Educ Behav. 2017;44(2):245–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Prakash RS, Voss MW, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Physical activity and cognitive vitality. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66(1):769–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Eddolls WTB, McNarry MA, Lester L, Winn CON, Stratton G, Mackintosh KA. The association between physical activity, fitness and body mass index on mental well-being and quality of life in adolescents. Qual Life Res. 2018;27(9):2313–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Nooijen CFJ, Stam HJ, Sluis T, Valent L, Twisk J, van den Berg-Emons RJG. A behavioral intervention promoting physical activity in people with subacute spinal cord injury: secondary effects on health, social participation and quality of life. Clin Rehabil. 2016;31(6):772–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Rosenman R, Tennekoon V, Hill LG. Measuring bias in self-reported data. Int J Behav Healthc Res. 2011;2(4):320–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. McArdle JJ. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;60(1):577–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Bullock JG, Green DP, Ha SE. Yes, but what’s the mechanism? (don’t expect an easy answer). J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;98(4):550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Cerin E, Barnett A, Baranowski T. Testing theories of dietary behavior change in youth using the mediating variable model with intervention programs. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2009;41(5):309–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Judd CM, Kenny DA. Process analysis: estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Eval Rev. 1981;5(5):602–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Mackinnon DP, Dwyer JH. Estimating mediated effects in prevention studies. Eval Rev. 1993;17(2):144–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable

Funding

The Active Team RCT was funded by the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia, project grant (APP1125913). CM is a recipient of a Career Development Fellowship from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia (APP1125913). The funder played no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, nor in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CM, RP, CV and TO conceived the original study. SE, JR and RC contributed to data collection. AR conducted the mediation analysis and led writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to interpretation, and drafting of the manuscript, and have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carol A. Maher.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval for the Active Team randomised controlled trial (RCT) was provided by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of South Australia, protocol number 0000033967. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Correlation Matrix.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Romeo, A.V., Edney, S.M., Plotnikoff, R.C. et al. Examining social-cognitive theory constructs as mediators of behaviour change in the active team smartphone physical activity program: a mediation analysis. BMC Public Health 21, 88 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-020-10100-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-020-10100-0

Keywords