Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Measuring commissioners’ willingness-to-pay for community based childhood obesity prevention programmes using a discrete choice experiment

Abstract

Background

In the UK, rates of childhood obesity remain high. Community based programmes for child obesity prevention are available to be commissioned by local authorities. However, there is a lack of evidence regarding how programmes are commissioned and which attributes of programmes are valued most by commissioners. The aim of this study was to determine the factors that decision-makers prioritise when commissioning programmes that target childhood obesity prevention.

Methods

An online discrete choice experiment (DCE) was used to survey commissioners and decision makers in the UK to assess their willingness-to-pay for childhood obesity programmes.

Results

A total of 64 commissioners and other decision makers completed the DCE. The impact of programmes on behavioural outcomes was prioritised, with participants willing to pay an extra £16,600/year if average daily fruit and vegetable intake increased for each child by one additional portion. Participants also prioritised programmes that had greater number of parents fully completing them, and were willing to pay an extra £4810/year for every additional parent completing a programme. The number of parents enrolling in a programme (holding the number completing fixed) and hours of staff time required did not significantly influence choices.

Conclusions

Emphasis on high programme completion rates and success increasing children’s fruit and vegetable intake has potential to increase commissioning of community based obesity prevention programmes.

Peer Review reports

Background

The number of children who start school with obesity in the UK has risen to 9.7% [1]. There are inequalities in prevalence, with children living in the most deprived areas more likely to be overweight or obese [1, 2]. Childhood obesity often persists into adulthood [3, 4], and due to related health issues, children with obesity can experience consequences as older children or adults. The consequences include effects on both physical health, e.g. pre-diabetes or high blood pressure [5], and mental health [6]. The UK government has set a goal to halve childhood obesity by 2030 [7].

In the UK, local authorities (a level of regional government) have a mandate to improve the health of the community. As part of addressing this responsibility, commissioners within local authorities can choose to provide various public health programmes. Local strategies and health priorities influence the choice of programmes that are commissioned. Childhood obesity is prioritised in many areas, with prevention in the early years now recognised as a key strategy [8, 9]. Programmes targeting obesity prevention in preschool children vary in their approach, including interventions aiming to modify food preferences (e.g. through repeated exposure to fruit and vegetables [10, 11]), parent focused interventions (e.g. [12, 13]) and behaviour change interventions directly targeting children [14].

Local authorities have limited resources, and every pound spent on childhood obesity programmes is a pound not spent on other policy areas, so commissioners and others involved in the process must carefully assess whether programmes are cost-effective. However, effectiveness evidence for many obesity prevention programmes is lacking. Thus, judgements may be based on other factors, such as likely success of implementation as assessed by monitoring data [15].

Further, there is a lack of published evidence on the commissioning process and the factors prioritised by decision-makers [15, 16], without which it is difficult to assess whether scarce resources are used optimally in practice. The current study attempted to address part of that knowledge gap by measuring decision-makers’ willingness-to-pay (WTP) for individual components of childhood obesity programmes using a discrete choice experiment (DCE) [17,18,19,20]. Decision-makers could include commissioners themselves, people who make recommendations about commissioning and people who help implement services. Although some previous research has looked at the commissioning of existing childhood obesity programmes [16, 21], the multicomponent nature of such programmes makes it difficult to disentangle what aspects and outcomes are most prioritised.

This study does not make any judgements as to whether the elicited preferences of commissioners and other decision-makers are appropriate, or whether they are likely to represent a good use of scarce resources. Instead, this study’s aim is to measure decision-makers’ priorities and the amounts they are in theory willing to pay to achieve given outcomes.

This work was conducted as part of a wider piece of research examining parental engagement with Health Exercise and Nutrition for the Really Young (HENRY), an early years obesity prevention programme in the UK [22, 23].

Methods

Survey development

In a DCE, participants are presented with a series of choices between hypothetical options, in this case which programme to provide. The options are described in terms of a set of attributes (e.g. number of parents enrolled, annual running cost). The levels the attributes take are varied in each question, and statistical analysis of their responses reveals the trade-offs participants make, e.g. more effective programmes versus increased cost of provision. A strength of the DCE methodology is that it allowed preferences for many different programmes to be estimated, including hypothetical programmes which do not currently exist, but could plausibly do so in future [24].

The attributes and levels selected for the DCE were guided by items/outcomes reported in previous evaluations [15, 25]. Initial attribute and level selection was done through unstructured discussion between authors (EW and WB) with expertise in health economics and childhood obesity until consensus was reached. Attributes and levels were finalised using unstructured discussion between authors (EW, WB, MC, HS, MB) until reaching consensus. We included six attributes, each with three levels (Table 1).

Table 1 Attributes and levels

The survey was designed to offer participants a choice in each task between two programmes consisting of differing attributes, rather than providing three or more different programmes to minimise task difficulty. An example question is shown in Fig. 1. Within each programme, parents would attend one session per week for eight weeks. Around two to three cohorts of parents would be able to attend each year per centre. Survey participants were informed that parents were defined as having successfully completed a programme if they attended at least five out of eight sessions. Many DCEs include an opt-out option, e.g. selecting 11 to provide neither programme, and providing an opt-out may influence participants’ choices [26, 27]. An opt-out was not included in this survey, as it was felt that minimising respondent burden as much as possible was important given the target population.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Example discrete choice experiment task

The statistical design of the DCE (i.e. which attribute levels are presented to participants in each question) was generated using the Ngene software package.Footnote 1 This program uses an algorithm to find a design that maximises D-efficiency, a standard statistic in the DCE literature, which may be thought of as a measure of how much information it is possible to gain from the responses [28]. More details, including the code used, are available in section 1 of the appendix. The final survey design, available in section 2 of the appendix, had 40 DCE tasks, which were split into four blocks so that each participant answered 10 questions, thus reducing the burden on respondents.

The survey was pilot tested with two commissioners who agreed that the attributes captured the most important features of childhood obesity programmes, that the survey was understandable, and that it presented an appropriate respondent burden.

Survey recruitment and administration

The survey was promoted using multiple approaches, so it is not possible to know how many times a given person was contacted as they may have received information through multiple channels. Direct emails were sent to relevant organisations and contacts, with an initial email and one reminder sent. Relevant organisations and contacts were: 151 members of the Association of Directors of Children’s Services in the UK and seven organisations (Obesity Health Alliance, Association of Directors of Public Health, Children’s Commissioner, Local Government Association, Public Health England, Health and Social Care Committee, HENRY commissioners). The study was advertised on Twitter and at a national Obesity Congress in the UK (UKCO2019). Two organisations publicised the DCE in their newsletters (Obesity Health Alliance, Association of Directors of Public Health), two organisations tweeted the link to the DCE (Public Health England with over 200,000 followers, HENRY with over 2000 followers), and one organisation promoted it on LinkedIn (HENRY). The link was also tweeted from the account of the Clinical Trials Research Unit at the University of Leeds with over 400 followers, and re-tweeted by authors. Responses were collected between May 2019 and September 2019.

The survey was administered using Jisc Online Surveys. Following participant consent, survey instructions were provided. Participants then completed the DCE tasks, and finally they answered questions to help characterise their roles. Full details are provided in an example survey in the appendix. Ethical approval for the study was given by the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds. Respondents were not paid, but a £3 donation to a national children’s charity (National Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Children, NSPCC) was made for every completed survey.

Analysis

Responses were examined to see if participants “straight-lined” the DCE tasks, either by always selecting Programme A or by always selecting Programme B. Responses were analysed using a random utility theory framework. This standard approach [29] assumed participants chose the programme which gave them the highest utility. The utility of a programme was modelled as partly depending on the attributes of the programme, and partly random. The random component captured the influence of all factors in decision-making not explicitly included in the model.

Analysis of DCE responses only gives information about participants’ relative preferences, that is, they can only show how much they prefer an attribute relative to another attribute, not their absolute preference for that attribute. The model was estimated in WTP space [30, 31], a term which means that preferences for all other variables are measured relative to participants’ preferences for running costs. The result is that the model coefficients for all other variables can be directly interpreted as participants’ marginal WTP in running costs for other attributes. The magnitude of the coefficient on running costs has no direct interpretation and is termed the numeraire. However, a positive sign may be interpreted as a preference for programmes with a lower running cost. Further detail about model estimation is available in the appendix.

The attribute average completion rate was presented to participants using both the rate (e.g. 50% of parents completing) and the absolute number (e.g. 5 parents completing), as it was uncertain which would be more relevant for participants. However, as the completion rate and the number of parents completing cannot vary independently, it is not possible to include both in a single model. Thus two separate models were estimated, one including the completion rate, and one including the number of parents completing. The final preferred model was then chosen as the one which minimised the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), a measure of how well a model fits data [32].

The results of model estimation can be used to calculate participants’ WTP in annual running costs for programmes with any given enrolment, completion, portions of fruit and vegetables, hours of staff time and setup costs. WTP was calculated for a range of hypothetical programmes. WTP was also estimated for three real world programmes, with the numbers completing, portions of fruit and vegetables, etc. based on results reported in the literature [12, 13, 33]. Note that these are intended as illustrative examples for the purpose of giving context only; they are not presented as a rigorous evaluation of the programmes. The exact numbers used to calculate WTPs, and their sources, are provided in section 3 of the appendix.

Analysis was carried out using the Apollo choice modelling package [34] run on R version 3.3.1. Statistical significance was assessed at the 5% level after adjustment using Holm’s sequential Bonferroni correction [35].

Results

A total of 112 people consented to participate, of whom 84 (75%) completed at least one DCE task, and 64 (57%) fully completed the survey. However, as responses were anonymous, it is possible that some individuals, who stopped part-way through the survey and then later returned to complete it, are counted twice. Only completed surveys were analysed. For completed surveys, answers to questions about respondent characteristics were examined for identical responses. None were found, which gives confidence that each completed survey is from a unique individual.

Respondents’ characteristics are summarised in Table 2. Over 80% of respondents were female. A large majority of respondents were of white ethnicity (84%), with the next largest ethnic group being Asian (9%). Respondents almost exclusively worked in England, with only 3% reporting working in Wales and none working in Scotland or Northern Ireland. Most areas in England were well represented, with at least five respondents from all areas except the East of England, which had four. The location with the greatest number of people working in it was London, with 13 participants.

Table 2 Participant demographics

Respondents’ professional roles are summarised in Table 3. Over half reported helping to implement services, with 45% directly commissioning services, and a slightly lower percentage making recommendations about what to commission. The median length of time participants had been in their role was four years, with six participants (9%) having been in their position for under a year, and 12 participants (19%) having been in position for 10 years or more.

Table 3 Participants’ roles

No participant was observed to straight-line the DCE tasks. Of the two models estimated, the model with the number of parents completing had a lower BIC, at 568.8, than the model with completion rate, which had a BIC of 574.5. Models with lower BIC are judged to fit the data better, and to give a better explanation of participants’ choices. The model with number of parents completing was chosen as the final preferred model, and only results from this are presented here. Results from the alternative model are available on request to the corresponding author.

Estimation results are shown in Table 4. The coefficient on annual running costs was significantly positive, indicating that participants were significantly more likely to prioritise programmes with lower running costs. In addition, participants expressed a preference for parental completion of programmes over that of initial recruitment; participants were willing to pay an additional £4810 per year for each additional parent per cohort who completed attendance. In contrast, the coefficient on the average number enrolling had a magnitude around 25 times smaller, and was statistically insignificant. (Note that as these figures are for a single cohort, a single extra parent completing per cohort implies an extra two to three completing each year.)

Table 4 Estimation results

Participants were significantly more likely to choose a programme that increased the number of portions of fruits and vegetables consumed by children. They were willing to pay an additional £16,600 if each child whose parent successfully completed a programme consumed on average one additional daily portion, holding all other variables constant. At 0.5 portions per completing parent, the lowest level presented to participants, they were willing to pay a total of £15,000 in setup costs and about £7500 in running costs for a programme with only two parents per cohort completing it.

Participants were also willing to pay around 3.5 times more for an improvement to a programme of one extra portion of fruit and vegetables eaten daily per child, holding all else constant, than for an improvement of one additional parent per cohort completing the programme, holding all else constant. More portions of fruit and vegetables per child mean more portions in total eaten if more parents complete the programme compared to fewer. Hence it was investigated whether participants were willing to pay an additional premium for programmes causing children to eat more portions of fruit and vegetables if more parents completed the programme. This was done by estimating a model with an interaction between responses for completion rate and fruits and vegetables. The interaction term was found to be insignificant (p = 0.697, full results available on request), hence there was no indication that participants were willing to pay any additional premium.

The coefficient on start-up costs was significant, negative, and has an absolute magnitude less than one. The interpretation is that participants prioritised running costs over start-up costs: participants were only willing to pay £0.39 in annual running costs for a £1 reduction in start-up cost. The additional number of staff hours required did not significantly influence choices, although the coefficient was negative, in line with expectations.

Figure 2 illustrated WTP for a range of hypothetical programmes, based on the results in Table 4. The attributes of average number of parents enrolling and hours of staff time were fixed at 66.7% and 8 h respectively, chosen as they are the midpoints of the attributes’ range. Also shown in the graphs are estimated WTP for three real world programmes.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Estimated willingness to pay for different programmes. Note. Average completion rate = 66.7%, Staff hours = 8

Discussion

Commissioners and related decision-makers in our study preferred programmes in which more parents completed them, with a WTP of over £4800 in running costs for each extra parent completing. In contrast, participants only expressed a weak preference for the number of parents enrolling. Thus the ability for programmes to retain participants is a key feature in whether decision-makers are willing to implement programmes. Recruiting more parents to a programme did not significantly alter our participants’ willingness to fund it unless it was possible to ensure that those parents also fully completed the programme.

Participants placed a high value on programmes that resulted in one extra portion of fruit and vegetables per day consumed by children, which may reflect the difficulties in achieving a full additional daily portion in a population. For example, several studies have shown that achieving a half extra portion per day is more achievable than one or two portions [36,37,38,39].

There are potential benefits of childhood obesity programmes not explicitly stated in the DCE attributes, for example physical activity or healthier eating habits. It is possible that some of the value placed on programmes by participants was due to these other benefits. Evidence for this is that there was considerable WTP for programmes with no effect on the number of portions of fruit and vegetables eaten. For example, with no additional portions and four parents completing, participants were still willing to pay £15,000 in setup costs and almost £9000 in running costs. Future research could usefully assess the value that decision-makers ascribe to outcomes such as encouraging exercise and improving eating habits.

Evidence from an ethnographic study within children’s centres [40] suggests that lack of staff time could be a barrier to implementing programmes from the perspective of service managers. This contrasts with the results of this study, that additional hours of staff time was not a major factor considered by commissioners and people supporting commissioning decisions over and above the broader financial costs, which were captured by other attributes. One potential reason for the difference between the perspectives of the participants from the two studies’ is that participants in the current study may not have been involved in the practical implementation of services. Future research could usefully examine differences in the perspectives of those involved in commissioning services and those managing them.

Setup costs were shown to substantially influence participants’ decision-making; however we found that annual running costs were valued more highly. This is expected, as the former is a one-off expense and the latter is a recurring expense, and so in the long run, the bulk of a programme’s cost will most likely be accounted for by annual running costs.

These results showed that participants were willing to pay large amounts of money for childhood obesity programmes. It is beyond the scope of this paper to assess whether this willingness to commit substantial funding is likely to result in an optimal use of resources, or whether the spending is cost-effective. However, it is hoped that the findings will inform a debate about whether current practice is in line with available evidence and what commissioning activity should focus on.

Previous studies have estimated the amount spent on behavioural weight management programmes for children, with figures of around £1300 [21] and £400 [21] per child completing. These figures are considerably lower than the WTP for the typical programme in our study, even when accounting for two to three cohorts attending per year. Yet the comparison is not straightforward, as the cited findings were for interventional programmes targeting overweight children, rather than the preventative programmes for parents evaluated in the current study. It may be that WTP was higher in this study due to prevention being prioritised over treatment, and future research could usefully explore this issue.

There is evidence that there is pressure on people involved in commissioning to reduce levels of childhood obesity [16]. While such pressure is not necessarily a bad thing, the high WTP found may reflect decision-makers feeling the need to take pro-active steps, even if they have limited impact. However, more research is required before being able to draw conclusions.

The outcomes of the hypothetical programmes were presented as certain to participants, so that when participants chose a programme they were guaranteed to achieve engagement and improvement in diet. For real-world programmes, achieving beneficial outcomes may be more uncertain, especially in the long term, in part due to a low evidence base [15, 16, 41, 42]. An alternative interpretation of a high WTP might be that the certainty associated with the hypothetical programmes meant they were perceived as representing a better use of resources than currently available programmes.

Although the behavioural outcomes of programmes were presented with certainty, the ultimate aim of the programmes was to reduce childhood obesity. No information on the impact of any behavioural change on obesity prevention was given to participants. Given the aforementioned low evidence base, the lack of information about the link between the amount of fruit and vegetables children eat and obesity may have made it difficult for participants to assess the value of programmes.

These results have highlighted some aspects of childhood obesity programmes that are prioritised by decision-makers and gives insights into what attributes they may wish to prioritise in monitoring and evaluation of programmes. However, this does not show which aspects are appropriate to emphasise in monitoring and evaluation in order to judge whether a programme represents a good use of resources. Further, our findings do not reflect the views of other stakeholders. Bryant et al. [15] conducted an exercise in which a range of stakeholders (including people involved in commissioning decisions) agreed on evaluation criteria for several public health interventions, including HENRY, a real-world programme similar to the hypothetical options presented to participants. The number of parents completing HENRY was not chosen as one of the top three evaluation criteria, in contrast to the preferences expressed by participants in this study. Further research could investigate potential differences in the priorities of commissioning decision-makers and other stakeholders.

We were able to apply our findings to estimate WTP for existing community-based obesity programmes, and found participants were willing to pay considerable amounts. For example, based on the findings of Willis et al. [36] participants would be willing to pay £15,000 in setup costs and £36,900 in annual running costs for the HENRY programme. Even with the MEND 2–4 programme, which had fewer than four parents completing, based on the results of Skouteris et al. [13] participants were willing to pay £15,000 in setup costs and £9600 in annual running costs. This exercise illustrates how the results could be used in future to assess whether new interventions are likely to be commissioned, analogous to the way in which DCEs have been included in predications of the uptake of medical treatments [43,44,45].

We are not aware of any previous DCEs that have targeted service commissioners and related decision-makers. Performing a DCE with this group raised potential challenges. For example, it was uncertain whether sufficient participants could be recruited, given a small target population with busy professional lives. It was also uncertain whether participants would find the choice tasks, involving deciding between providing alternative services, acceptable and meaningful. Participants’ preferences were in line with prior expectations (e.g. they preferred better outcomes and lower costs rather than the other way round), which is evidence that they interpreted the DCE tasks in the way they were intended and responded in a logical way. This study demonstrated that DCEs with service commissioners and related decision-makers are feasible, and future DCE studies could fruitfully elicit the preferences of similar populations.

This study has some limitations. It was not possible to assess how representative the sample was of all UK decision-makers, as almost all participants worked in England (albeit with representation from all areas of England). The sample was also self-selected and not randomly sampled, which means that participants may not be representative of the target population. For example, individuals who place a greater emphasis on childhood obesity prevention might both be more likely to value programmes and more likely to complete the survey. Over half of respondents did not directly commission services, meaning that although the results were indicative of the preferences of those supporting commissioning decisions in practice, they did not necessarily make funding decisions for implementation of services.

Our study sample size was small compared to many other similar studies [17]. The total of 64 participants is also somewhat lower than rules-of-thumb in the DCE literature, for example Johnson and Orme [46] recommend 75 and Lancsar and Louviere [47] recommend 80 given the survey design used here. However, the sample is likely to represent a reasonable fraction of all possible respondents in the UK given the specialised nature of the target population. In particular, it is likely to represent a large fraction of commissioners given over half of participants reported performing that role. Additionally, the numbers recruited allowed robust, advanced statistical models to be estimated.

It is often considered good practice to work closely with stakeholders representing the participant population when developing DCE surveys and deciding attributes [48, 49]. Our input was limited to consultation with two commissioners and obesity experts during the survey development. This may be justified by the fact that our attributes had largely been chosen from previous literature in this area [15, 23, 36]; though it may be a limitation that the construction of attributes and levels was heavily influenced by existing literature on a specific programme. In addition, participants were professionals, rather than patients, making it easier for authors to assess how appropriate the language of attributes and levels were for the target population.

This study presents participants’ stated preferences (i.e. statements about what they would do in a hypothetical decision-making situation). Concerns have been raised over how externally valid WTP estimates based on stated preference are (e.g. [50, 51]). For example, some studies have found that WTP estimates are subject to hypothetical bias [52, 53], meaning that individuals overstate how much they would pay compared to a real situation. This could be a reason that the WTPs measured here exceed previous estimates of the amounts spent on programmes in reality [16, 21]. Other studies suggest that the range of levels in a DCE can affect WTP [54], for example presenting a higher range of costs may increase WTP.

Conclusions

This study has demonstrated that people involved in commissioning services state that they are willing to pay substantial amounts for the provision of programmes to prevent childhood obesity in preschool settings. In particular, they may be willing to pay more for programmes that have higher numbers of parents fully completing and successfully result in children eating more portions of fruit and vegetables.

Future research could build on the current findings. It would be beneficial to combine the stated preference data gathered here with real-world data, as Buckell and Hess [48] and Wuepper, Clemm and Wree [49] have done, in order to improve the external validity of findings. It would also be interesting to compare the preferences of commissioners to the preferences of those implementing services on the ground, as well as service users.

Availability of data and materials

Data is not publically available, as consent for this was not obtained. However, data may be made available on a case by case basis via a formal data sharing agreement either by contacting Leeds Institute of Health Sciences, University of Leeds, UK, or by contacting the corresponding author directly.

Notes

  1. ©ChoiceMetrics

Abbreviations

DCE:

Discrete choice experiment

WTP:

Willingness-to-pay

HENRY:

Health Exercise and Nutrition for the Really Young

MEND:

Mind, Exercise, Nutrition … Do It!

BIC:

Bayesian information criterion

References

  1. Public Health England, National Child Measurement Programme. 2019. https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/national-child-measurement-programme.

  2. McSweeney LA, et al. Perceptions of nursery staff and parent views of healthy eating promotion in preschool settings: an exploratory qualitative study. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):841.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Oude Luttikhuis H. et al, Cochrane review: Interventions for treating obesity in children. Evidence-based Child Health. A Cochrane Rev J. 2009;4(4):1571–729.

  4. Morandi A, et al. Estimation of newborn risk for child or adolescent obesity: lessons from longitudinal birth cohorts. PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e49919.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Sharma V, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis estimating the population prevalence of comorbidities in children and adolescents aged 5 to 18 years. Obes Rev. 2019;20(10):1341–9.

  6. Morrison KM, et al. Association of depression & health related quality of life with body composition in children and youth with obesity. J Affect Disord. 2015;172:18–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. HM Government, Childhood obesity: a plan for action. 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Lumeng JC, et al. Changes in body mass index associated with head start participation. Pediatrics. 2015;135(2):e449–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Waters E, et al. Interventions for preventing obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2011;12. https://www.cochranelibrary.com/cdsr/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD001871.pub2/full.

  10. Wardle J, et al. Modifying children's food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2003;57(2):341.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Anzman-Frasca S, et al. Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children’s liking of vegetables. Appetite. 2012;58(2):543–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Martínez-Andrade GO, et al. Feasibility and impact of Creciendo Sanos, a clinic-based pilot intervention to prevent obesity among preschool children in Mexico City. BMC Pediatr. 2014;14(1):77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Skouteris H, et al. A parent-based intervention to promote healthy eating and active behaviours in pre-school children: evaluation of the MEND 2–4 randomized controlled trial. Pediatric Obesity. 2016;11(1):4–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Başkale H, Bahar Z. Outcomes of nutrition knowledge and healthy food choices in 5-to 6-year-old children who received a nutrition intervention based on Piaget's theory. J Specialists Pediatric Nurs. 2011;16(4):263–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Bryant M, et al. Use of progression criteria to support monitoring and commissioning decision making of public health services: lessons from better start Bradford. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):835.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Mears R, et al. Exploring how lifestyle weight management programmes for children are commissioned and evaluated in England: a mixed methodology study. BMJ Open. 2019;9(12):e025423.

  17. Soekhai V, et al. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: past, present and future. Pharmacoeconomics. 2019;37(2):201–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Clark MD, et al. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: a review of the literature. Pharmacoeconomics. 2014;32(9):883–902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. de Bekker-Grob EW, Ryan M, Gerard K. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: a review of the literature. Health Econ. 2012;21(2):145–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ryan M, Gerard K. Using discrete choice experiments to value health care programmes: current practice and. Appl Health Econ Health Policy. 2003;2(1):55–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Coulton, V., et al., National mapping of weight management services: provision of tier 2 and tier 3 services in England. 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Bryant M, et al. Cluster randomised controlled feasibility study of HENRY: a community-based intervention aimed at reducing obesity rates in preschool children. Pilot Feasibility Studies. 2018;4(118):1–15.

  23. Bryant M, et al. Effectiveness of an implementation optimisation intervention aimed at increasing parent engagement in HENRY, a childhood obesity prevention programme - the Optimising family engagement in HENRY (OFTEN) trial: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials. 2017;18(1):40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Ryan M. Discrete choice experiments in health care. BMJ. 2004;328.

  25. Bryant M, et al. Effectiveness of an implementation optimisation intervention aimed at increasing parent engagement in HENRY, a childhood obesity prevention programme-the Optimising family engagement in HENRY (OFTEN) trial: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials. 2017;18(1):40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Veldwijk J, et al. The effect of including an opt-out option in discrete choice experiments. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e111805.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Campbell D, Erdem S. Including opt-out options in discrete choice experiments: issues to consider. Patient-Patient-Centered Outcomes Res. 2019;12(1):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Kuhfeld WF, Tobias RD, Garratt M. Efficient experimental design with marketing research applications. J Mark Res. 1994;31(4):545–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Louviere JJ, Hensher DA, Swait JD. Stated Choice Methods: Analysis and Applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  30. Train K, Weeks M. Discrete choice models in preference space and willingness-to-pay space, in Applications of simulation methods in environmental and resource economics. New York: Springer; 2005. p. 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Hole AR, Kolstad JR. Mixed logit estimation of willingness to pay distributions: a comparison of models in preference and WTP space using data from a health-related choice experiment. Empir Econ. 2012;42(2):445–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat. 1978;6(2):461–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Brown RE, et al. Preventing child obesity: a long-term evaluation of the HENRY approach. Community Practitioner. 2013;86(7):23–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hess S, Palma D. Apollo: a flexible, powerful and customisable freeware package for choice model estimation and application. J Choice Modelling. 2019;32:100170.

  35. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. 1979;6(2):65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Willis T, et al. The impact of HENRY on parenting and family lifestyle: a national service evaluation of a preschool obesity prevention programme. Public Health. 2016;136:101–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Evans CE, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of school-based interventions to improve daily fruit and vegetable intake in children aged 5 to 12 y. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(4):889–901.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Christian MS, et al. Evaluation of the impact of a school gardening intervention on children’s fruit and vegetable intake: a randomised controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(1):99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Ransley J, et al. Does the school fruit and vegetable scheme improve children’s diet? A non-randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61(8):699–703.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Burton W, et al. Participant engagement with a UK community-based preschool childhood obesity prevention programme: a focused ethnography study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):1074.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Hodder RK, et al. Interventions for increasing fruit and vegetable consumption in children aged five years and under. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018;5. https://www.cochranelibrary.com/cdsr/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD008552.pub7/full.

  42. Upton P, et al. Family-based childhood obesity interventions in the UK: a systematic review of published studies. Community Practitioner. 2014;87(5):25–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Quaife M, et al. How well do discrete choice experiments predict health choices? A systematic review and meta-analysis of external validity. Eur J Health Econ. 2018;19(8):1053–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ostermann J, et al. Preferences for health interventions: improving uptake, adherence, and efficiency. Patient-Patient-Centered Outcomes Res. 2017;10(4):511–4.

  45. Terris-Prestholt F, Quaife M, Vickerman P. Parameterising user uptake in economic evaluations: the role of discrete choice experiments. Health Econ. 2016;25:116–23.

  46. Johnson R. Orme B. Getting the most from CBC. Sequim. Sawtooth Software: Sawtooth Software Research Paper Series; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Lancsar E, Louviere J. Conducting discrete choice experiments to inform healthcare decision making. Pharmacoecon. 2008;26(8):661–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Coast J, et al. Using qualitative methods for attribute development for discrete choice experiments: issues and recommendations. Health Econ. 2012;21(6):730–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Kløjgaard ME, Bech M, Søgaard R. Designing a stated choice experiment: the value of a qualitative process. J Choice Modelling. 2012;5(2):1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Harrison GW. Real choices and hypothetical choices. In: Hess S, Daly A, editors. Handbook of Choice Modelling. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar; 2014. p. 236.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Fifer S, Rose J, Greaves S. Hypothetical bias in stated choice experiments: is it a problem? And if so, how do we deal with it? Transp Res A Policy Pract. 2014;61:164–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Buckell J, Hess S. Stubbing out hypothetical bias: improving tobacco market predictions by combining stated and revealed preference data. J Health Econ. 2019;65:93–102.

  53. Wuepper D, Clemm A, Wree P. The preference for sustainable coffee and a new approach for dealing with hypothetical bias. J Econ Behav Organ. 2019;158:475–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Rakotonarivo OS, Schaafsma M, Hockley N. A systematic review of the reliability and validity of discrete choice experiments in valuing non-market environmental goods. J Environ Manag. 2016;183:98–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

EW would like to thank Prof Sandy Tubeuf, Université Catholique de Louvain for support in conceiving the study.

Funding

This study was funded by the NIHR Academy Programme awarded to Maria Bryant (co-author) The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The funder had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data or writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EW conceived the study. EW, WB, MC, HS and MB constructed attributes and levels. EW programmed the survey. ES recruited participants. EW analysed data. All authors contributed to interpreting results. EW wrote the manuscript first draft. All authors contributed substantial revisions to the manuscript and approved the final draft.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward J. D. Webb.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the University of Leeds Faculty of Medicine and Health Research Ethics Committee (reference MREC 15–017) and participants gave informed consent to take part electronically by clicking a button at the start of the survey.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

Survey statistical design, model estimation, calculating willingness to pay for real world programmes.

Additional file 2.

Survey on childhood obesity programmes. Example survey.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Webb, E.J.D., Stamp, E., Collinson, M. et al. Measuring commissioners’ willingness-to-pay for community based childhood obesity prevention programmes using a discrete choice experiment. BMC Public Health 20, 1535 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-020-09576-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-020-09576-7

Keywords