Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Which interventions increase hearing protection behaviors during noisy recreational activities? A systematic review

Abstract

Background

Hearing loss and tinnitus are global concerns that can be reduced through hearing protection behaviors (e.g., earplug use). Little is known about the effectiveness of interventions to increase hearing protection use in recreational domains. For the first time we review systematically the effectiveness of such interventions.

Methods

Systematic searches of nine databases, as well as grey literature and hand-searching, were conducted. Any study design was included if it assessed quantitatively a purposeful attempt to increase hearing protection in recreational settings. Studies were excluded if they assessed noise exposure from occupational sources and headphones/earphones, as these have been reviewed elsewhere. PROSPERO protocol: CRD42018098573.

Results

Eight studies were retrieved following the screening of 1908 articles. Two pretest-posttest studies detected a small to medium effect (d ≥ 0·3 ≤ 0·5), one a small effect (d ~ =0·2) and two no real effect. Three posttest experimental studies detected small to medium effects (d ≥ 0·3 ≤ 0·5). Studies were rated as “poor quality” and 17 out of a possible 93 behavior change techniques were coded, with the majority targeting the intervention function ‘education’.

Conclusions

Hearing loss and tinnitus due to recreational noise exposure are major public health concerns yet very few studies have examined preventive interventions. The present systematic review sets the agenda for the future development and testing of evidence-based interventions designed to prevent future hearing loss and tinnitus caused by noise in recreational settings, by recommending systematic approaches to intervention design, and implementation of intervention functions beyond education, such as incentivization, enablement and modeling.

Peer Review reports

Background

Approximately one billion teenagers and young adults (12–35 years) are at risk of noise-induced hearing loss and tinnitus due to hazardous recreational noise exposure [1]. Recreational activities such as attendance at live music venues (nightclubs, festivals, concerts and bars), practising/producing music, do-it-yourself (DIY), engine noise and sports related noise [2,3,4,5,6] contribute the majority of risk, [7] with noise levels ranging between 91.7–140 dBA (A-weighted decibel), [8,9,10,11,12] and depending on duration of exposure all have the potential to cause hearing symptoms in a short space of time [13, 14]. Individuals who partake in recreational activities are more likely to have hearing loss than those who do not, [15] with dullness in hearing and tinnitus reported in up to 80% of people post activity [16, 17]. Recreational noise exposure can be reduced through the adoption of hearing protection behaviors, such as the use of hearing protection devices (earplugs and earmuffs) and regeneration breaks [18, 19]. However, people engaging in hearing protection behaviors during noisy recreational activities has been reported as fewer than 5%, [20, 21] and it is not known whether interventions have exerted measurable effect sizes post intervention in changing behaviors.

Previous hearing protection systematic and narrative reviews have investigated occupational settings, [22] recreational noise through personal listening devices that use headphones and earphones, [23] and education about hearing protection [24]. El Dib et al. [22] conducted a systematic review of randomised controlled trials in occupational settings designed to promote the wearing of earplugs and earmuffs and concluded that specifically tailored or individual based education interventions improved use of hearing protection. Diviani et al’s [23] narrative systematic review of personal listening devices identified two interventions [25, 26] indicating that warning signs and evocative imagery reduced volume levels. Kahn et al’s [24] systematic review focused on health education programmes targeted at youth and young adults’ use of hearing protection in occupational and recreational settings, identifying 10 studies that showed little evidence of effectiveness. However, given that education is just one out of nine possible functions that an intervention might serve (see Michie et al. [27]), it would be valuable to examine the effects of other intervention functions, such as environmental restructuring, modelling and incentivization as potential means to bring about health protection behavior change among people of all ages.

The use of health psychology theories and models have been discussed as a means to improving hearing health behavior change interventions, [28] with Coulson et al. [29] suggesting the ‘behavior change wheel’ framework and associated capability (C), opportunity (O) and motivation (M) model of behavior (B) change (COM-B) [27] as a new approach to use within this domain. The COM-B model is at the core of the framework, with capability, opportunity and motivation representative of the processes involved in enacting a behavior [27]. The COM-B model allows intervention designers to assess which drivers of the target behavior need to change. Once this has been established, then the remaining steps of the behavior change wheel framework help refine the components required for the target intervention, including intervention functions (categories of intervention), the behavior change techniques (active ingredients of the intervention), and the mode of delivering the final intervention [27].

The 93 techniques in the ‘Behavior Change Technique Taxonomy Version 1’ (BCTTv1) [30] are the smallest active ingredients of interventions and act as the catalysts energizing the appropriately identified intervention functions during the design process of the behavior change wheel [27]. For example, the technique ‘demonstration of the behavior (BCTTv1:6.1)’ would serve the intervention functions of both education and modelling. In previous preventative health behavior systematic reviews (e.g., physical activity) commonly used behavior change techniques including ‘goal setting (behavior) (BCTTv1:1.1)’, [31] and ‘feedback on behavior (BCTTv1:2.2)’ have been coded [32]. The coding of the behavior change techniques within hearing protection interventions will aid future intervention designers during this theory driven process. However, none of the previous hearing protection systematic [22, 24] and narrative reviews [23] have coded interventions to identify the behavior change techniques implemented according to the BCTTv1 taxonomy, [30] alongside extracting measurable hearing protection use outcome effects (Cohen’s d).

The present systematic review looks beyond occupational settings, includes hearing protection behaviors beyond personal listening device use, considers intervention functions in addition to education (e.g., incentivization [27]), and codes behavior change techniques for the first time in recreational hearing protection interventions. The aim of the present research is therefore to review systematically the literature on interventions designed to increase hearing protection behaviors in recreational settings.

The primary objectives of the current review are to: 1) quantify the effectiveness of hearing protection interventions in recreational noise domains, and 2) identify the active ingredients (behavior change techniques) of such interventions to help future designers of interventions to increase uptake and use of hearing protection behaviors in recreational settings.

Methods

Search strategy and selection criteria

This is a systematic review that followed the ‘Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses’ guidance, [33] which was pre-registered on PROSPERO [CRD42018098573] on 6th June 2018. No meta-analysis could take place due to study heterogeneity. https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=98573.

Searches were carried out on electronic databases: Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials; PubMED; EMBASE; MEDLINE; PsycINFO; Web of Science Core Collection; ComDisDome; Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects and Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. Grey literature was searched via: Grey Literature Report, Prospero, Open Grey, ClinicalTrials.gov, International Clinical Trials Registry. Hand-searching was conducted using the reference lists of papers that were included for full review. Authors of 12 studies were contacted for further clarification before inclusion or exclusion could be determined. The most recent full search was performed on 1st May 2020. All age groups, years and languages were considered for review.

A broad search strategy was developed alongside a research librarian to capture the large variety of hearing protection interventions (hearing conservation programs, education programs, hearing protection device use and noise legislation adherence). Keywords and structure to each search were altered depending on each electronic database (see Additional file 1); each strategy followed components related to intervention types, effects of noise exposure, hearing protection and different sound sources. Medical Subject Heading (MeSH) terms in this instance were not used due to the close association between occupational and recreational noise.

Studies were included if they assessed quantitatively a deliberate attempt to increase hearing protection behaviors when people are exposed to noise during recreational activities; study designs included ‘experimental posttest designs’ (randomized controlled trials and quasi-experimental studies) and ‘single group pretest-posttest designs’ (observational studies and surveys). Secondary outcomes included hearing health outcomes such as hearing loss or tinnitus as well as perceptions of capabilities, opportunities and motivations to engage in hearing protective behaviors. Interventions were also coded for the presence of the active ingredients aimed at delivering the desired change, namely, the behavior change techniques. The 93 techniques are clustered into 16 groups (e.g., goals and planning, reward and threat) within the behavior change technique taxonomy (BCTTv1), each technique has its own identifiable number for coding purposes, with the first digit identifying the cluster group, and the second the order of the technique within said group (e.g., habit formation: BCTTv1:8.1) [30]. These techniques were required to be observable, replicable, irreducible and to include a postulated active ingredient of the intervention. They were also required to be clearly defined to the target population(s) and target behavior(s)/outcome(s) within study methodologies [27, 30]. Coding the included studies clarified any effects of the intervention on primary and secondary outcomes. Studies were excluded if they assessed noise exposure from occupational sources and noise from personal listening devices while using headphones and/or earphones, as these have been reviewed elsewhere [22, 23].

The screening of abstracts and titles commenced with the 1st reviewer (ML) screening all articles and 2nd reviewer (SL) screening 10% of these reports for comparison. The 1st reviewer (ML) read all the fully eligible studies, with the 2nd and 3rd reviewers (SL and CJA) reading a proportion of the eligible studies, as well as discussing any query papers for full inclusion. Several authors were contacted directly to clarify information, leading to both inclusion and exclusion of studies for full review.

Data extraction and analysis

Data were extracted using a pre-designed and piloted data extraction form, including general study information, study characteristics, participant characteristics, intervention design (including behavior change techniques), and outcome measures (see Table 1 and Additional file 2). Risk of bias and quality assessment were carried out independently by the 1st and 2nd reviewers using the Cochrane Quality Assessment Tool for systematic reviews [42]. The published protocol stated that the review would use the critical appraisal skills program (CASP) [43, 44]. However, with the included study designs not conforming to CASP checklists, and high levels of heterogeneity meaning determining the level of evidence using Cochrane’s ‘Grading of Recommendations, Assessment, Development and Evaluations’ tool was not possible, it was more appropriate to use the Cochrane Quality Assessment Tool for systematic reviews (see Fig. 1).

Table 1 Inclusion table with effect estimates and behavior change techniques
Fig. 1
figure 1

Cochrane Risk of Bias Table

Outcome measures were assessed in numerous different formats (often with multiple categories) and so the main outcome was recoded as ‘never-performers’ (never performed a hearing protection behavior) and ‘ever-performers’ (performed a hearing protection behavior at least some of the time). Quantitative data were extracted by the 1st and 2nd reviewer, and compared for agreement. The 3rd reviewer checked over the extracted quantitative data for agreement and calculation errors. One paper written in German was translated and included in the analysis [39]. Where studies included multiple time points, data were extracted from the last follow-up.

For dichotomous data, risk ratios and Z statistics were extracted; Cohen’s d was calculated using Z statistic and sample size (N) [45]. In terms of risk ratios, “ever-performers” of hearing protection behaviors were coded ‘positive outcomes,’ and “never-performers” were coded ‘negative outcomes.’ The cumulative incidence of the intervention group (or posttest data) was then divided by the cumulative incidence of the control group (or pretest data) (see Table 1). This approach is similar to that applied in the previous systematic review of occupational hearing protection behaviors [22]. One study presented data as adjusted means for the proportion of time hearing protection was used [38]; Cohen’s d could be inferred from the presented adjusted means and sample sizes provided [45].

The 1st and 2nd reviewers, trained via the University College London online behavior change technique taxonomy v1 program, [46] coded all included papers for behavior change techniques with an 80% agreement rate and a Cohen’s Kappa moderate agreement (κ = 0·58). The 3rd reviewer coded any disagreements and from this the final list of codes were agreed between all reviewers. Full coding of all behavior change techniques and associated taxonomy numbering can been found in Table 1.

Results

Initial searching recovered 2616 articles, of which 1908 (73%) were eligible for screening after the removal of duplicates (n = 708). Following screening, a total of 59 reports were eligible for full review, eight of which were suitable for inclusion according to our criteria (see Fig. 2). Five of these were single group pretest-posttest designs and three were experimental post-test designs (two randomized controlled trials and one quasi-randomized study). Fifty-one papers were excluded due to: studies missing or no behavioral data (n = 14); measurement of ‘intended behavior’ and not actual behavior (n = 13); no recognizable intervention (n = 11); occupational noise exposure (n = 6); unretrievable data (n = 5); acoustic intervention (n = 1); and contained historic data superseded by a subsequent paper (n = 1).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Flow Diagram

Individual effects were extracted for each study and are discussed in a narrative synthesis. Study heterogeneity occurred due to study design differences (five single group pretest-posttest designs and three experimental posttest designs), a range of follow up periods (16 weeks to 16 years) and a large population range (39 participants up to 1535 participants). All studies examined use of earplugs, one study additionally measured regeneration breaks alongside earplugs, [41] and a further study additionally measured adjustments of personal stereo volume through loudspeakers [38].

Seven of the included interventions were described as hearing conservation/education programs, [34, 36,37,38,39,40,41] with the final intervention focused solely on provision of free hearing protection devices (earplugs) versus no provision [35]. School children were the target of five studies, [36, 38,39,40,41] young adults (18–39 years) the target for two studies, [34, 37] and the final study did not define an age group or report an average age [35].

Single pretest-posttest design studies

When examining the five single pretest-posttest designs we compared baseline data with final follow-up. Neyen’s [39] hearing conservation program delivered two hearing health teaching sessions, which included explanations of hearing loss (information about health consequences: BCTTv1: 5.1), audio simulations (salience of consequences: BCTTv1: 5.2) and experiments with volume levels (comparative imaging of future outcomes: BCTTv1: 9.3). The study detected only a very small increase in uptake of earplugs (d = 0·14) during loud events with German school children in the 5–6 weeks following the intervention. No secondary outcomes were reported. The study had high risk of allocation concealment bias as there was no randomization, and detection bias (blinding of outcomes) as blinding was not mentioned.

Weichbold and Zorowka’s [40] hearing conservation program “PROjectEAR” detected a small effect (d = 0·21) with increased use of earplugs at discotheques within Austrian high school children, 1 year post intervention. The children received four teaching sessions that included information on the risks of noise exposure (information about health consequences; BCTTv1: 5.1) alongside multimedia/listening examples (salience of consequences: BCTTv1: 5.2), and presentation of ear protection devices (instruction on how to perform a behavior: BCTTv1: 4.1; demonstration of the behavior: BCTTv1: 6.1). No secondary outcomes were reported. The study had high risk of allocation concealment bias, performance bias, and detection bias (blinding of outcomes), due to no randomization or mention of blinding.

Weichbold and Zorowka [41] continued with “PROjectEAR”, this time with a new and larger sample size of students (see Additional file 2) and an additional behavior measuring regeneration breaks. Additionally this version clearly stated the children received a talk from a hearing impaired person (credible source; BCTTv1: 9.1), with instruction to practice using earplugs (behavioral practice/rehearsal; BCTTv1: 8.1), and to remove themselves from noisy spaces (restructuring the physical environment; BCTTv1: 12.1); however, salience of consequences (BCTTv1: 5.2) was not explicitly coded this time around. The study detected a very small increase (d = 0·14) in earplug use 1 year post intervention, but no real effect (d = 0.06) of increased regeneration breaks. There are no reported secondary outcomes within this paper. Similarly, as within Weichbold and Zorowka [40] this study had high risk of allocation concealment bias and detection bias (blinding of outcomes).

Keppler et al.’s [37] hearing conservation program was performed by an audiologist (credible source; BCTTv1: 9.1) whom delivered feedback on hearing (biofeedback: BCTTv1: 2.6), educated on the risks of recreational noise, and discussed protective actions, including benefits/barriers (information about health consequences: BCTTv1: 5.1; information about social and environmental consequences: BCTTv1: 5.3: instruction on how to perform a behavior: BCTTv1: 4.1; pros and cons: BCTTv1: 9.2). The study detected a small to medium effect (d = 0·34) in increased use of hearing protection devices from baseline (mean 3·40; SD 1·36; range 1·00–5·00) to 6 months post intervention (mean 2·94; SD 1·37; range 1·00–5·00) within Belgian young adults. Secondary outcomes assessed the audiometric thresholds of the participants at baseline and 6 months post intervention, but no significant effects were found. Participant self-reports between sessions indicated that 28·2% of participants perceived their hearing loss to have increased, with 20·5% reporting that their tinnitus increased. However, the study had high risk of allocation concealment bias, performance bias, and detection bias, due to no randomization or mention of blinding.

The aim of Gilles and Van de Heyning’s [36] hearing conservation program was to make students aware of the dangers of loud music (information about health consequences: BCTTv1: 5.1) and therefore increase use of hearing protection; after administering questionnaires to Belgian students at baseline and 6 months post intervention, they detected a small to medium effect (d = 0·34) in increased use of devices while in noisy recreational environments. There are no reported secondary outcomes within this paper. The study was high risk of allocation concealment bias, performance bias, and detection bias, due to no randomization or mention of blinding.

Experimental post-test designs

When examining the three experimental post-test designs we compared control and intervention data at the final follow-up. Marlenga et al. [38] completed a 16-year follow up of a hearing conservation program that was originally a clustered randomized controlled trial of rural American school children. Self-reported hearing protection use revealed a small to medium effect (d = 0·30) in the difference between groups for hearing protection use during gunfire. However, no effect was found for ‘all recreational activities’ (d = 0·07) or ‘personal stereos’ (d = 0·03). The program consisted of information on the ear and hearing delivered by a study educator, alongside videotape examples (credible source: BCTTv1: 9.1; information about health consequences; BCTTv1: 5.1; salience of consequences: BCTTv1: 5.2), and demonstrations/practice of how to fit hearing protection devices (demonstration of the behavior: BCTTv1: 6.1; behavioral practice/rehearsal: BCTTv1: 8.1); concluding with the provision of free devices that continued over the course of the intervention, alongside additional information (adding objects to the environment: BCTTv1: 12.5; habit formation: BCTTv1: 8.3). Secondary outcomes assessed changes in audiometric threshold frequencies from baseline across five categories, however no significant differences between groups were found for any of the categories. The study had high risk of detection bias as there is no mention of blinding, attrition bias due to dropouts, and reporting bias due to collection methods not known to be valid or reliable. There was also a high risk of selection bias due to a low agreement rate at the 16 year follow up, with an overall low risk of allocation concealment bias through initial randomization.

Beach et al.’s [34] hearing conservation program had the incentive of free earplugs and use demonstrated by an audiologist (material incentive (behavior): BCTTv1: 10.1; adding objects to the environment: BCTTv1: 12.5; credible source: BCTTv1: 9.1; instruction on how to perform a behavior: BCTTv1: 4.1; demonstration of the behavior: BCTTv1: 6.1), with a monetary reward for intervention completion (material reward (behavior): BCTTv1: 10.2). However, the experimental group received additional information on the dangers of noise, a video demonstrating hearing loss, and additional time with the audiologist (information about health consequences: BCTTv1: 5.1; salience of consequences: BCTTv1: 5.2; social support (practical): (BCTTv1: 3.2). The study detected a small to medium effect (d = 0·30) in differences of earplug use at live music events at 16 weeks post intervention within Australian young adults who regular attended live events. However, it is worth noting for this study that 82% of participants had used earplugs previously. This is unusually high and not representative of the general population, which is 5% for these types of events [21, 47]. Secondary outcomes were assessed with self-reporting of temporary thresholds shifts (76%) and tinnitus (92%) among all participants at follow up, with tinnitus reported as being permanent in 20% of the participants. The study reported as high risk of detection bias (blinding of outcome assessment) as it does not mention blinding of assessors, but with good internal validity due to randomization and blinding of participants.

When comparing control venues (three concerts) to experimental venues (three concerts) Cha et al. [35] detected a small to medium effect (d = 0·31) with greater use of earplugs being observed when earplugs were freely available at rock concerts (prompts and cues: BCTTv1: 7.1; material incentive (behavior): BCTTv1: 10.1). However, no secondary outcomes were measured meaning that it is not clear what was the mechanism of action. We considered this to be a quasi-study as it did not randomize participants but instead had control/interventions groups defined by venues, and was therefore high risk of allocation concealment bias; with large discrepancies in sample size between comparison groups. The study also had high risk of detection bias, attrition bias, and reporting bias, due to no mention of blinding or dropouts/withdrawals, and collection methods not known to be valid or reliable.

Discussion

This review set out to assess, the effectiveness of recreational hearing protection interventions, and for the first time, to identify the active ingredients of these interventions. Considering hearing symptoms through recreational noise exposure is a global concern that is highly preventable, [1] only eight studies were retrieved that evaluated changes in hearing protection behaviors post intervention; only three studies have been carried out within the last 5 years, [34, 35, 37] indicating hearing protection interventions are being chronically under researched. Included studies lacked quality overall, with only three experimental post-test designs (two randomized controlled trials and one quasi study). Methodologies and results were poorly reported making it difficult to extract data, resulting in 12 authors being contacted throughout the review process. Furthermore, the poor quality of reporting highlights how difficult it would be, if not impossible, to replicate many of the interventions to further test outcomes. There is a need for more high quality experimental studies; improvements are required in terms of design quality and reporting to address this large gap in knowledge. This review included three studies [36,37,38] that were included in a previous systematic review, [24] but differed in interpretation because effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and behavior change techniques were extracted. Throughout screening it was apparent that interventions tended to measure people’s intentions to protect their hearing, as opposed to their actual hearing protection behaviors, [48,49,50,51,52] with a total of 13 papers rejected on this basis. Intentions do not represent the true effect of an intervention as people fail to act on their intentions approximately 50% of the time, [53] and this is an area that needs to be addressed with future hearing protection interventions.

The most common hearing protection behavior reported in each study was the use of earplugs. This supports previous evidence that hearing protection devices are the most accessible preventive method against overexposure to recreational noise [18, 48, 54]. While examining the effectiveness of the studies five indicated a small to medium effect (Cohen’s d), measuring an increase, or difference, in earplug use across a range of recreational contexts. However, those five studies had strong risk of bias, particularly detection bias (blinding of outcome assessment), with poorly reported methodologies affecting replication credentials.

The overall increase in ‘ever-performers’ of earplugs across all eight studies ranged from 3% – 14·6%; few studies indicate people always or often use earplugs, indicating many people are at risk of hearing symptoms in a short space of time, [13, 14] when these activities often have noise levels recorded at over 100 dBA [10,11,12]. Beach et al. [34] was the outlier of the group with 90% of all participants ‘ever-performers’; however, 82% were ‘ever-performers’ pre-study, which was perhaps due to targeting regular gig goers with an incentive of free earplugs. The present systematic review also highlights that preventative behaviors such as regeneration breaks, keeping safe distances, lowering sound levels, and adherence to legislations are less frequently addressed and should be avenues for future research.

Unique to the present systematic review was the coding of behavior change techniques deployed within hearing protection interventions, with a total of 17 coded as involving behavior change from a possible 93 techniques, the majority of which link to the intervention function ‘education’ (information about health consequences: BCTTv1:5.1). With so few techniques being identified it leaves a large scope to deploy previously untested techniques to bring about changes in hearing protection behaviors. Coding did reveal other less frequently used intervention functions, such as ‘environmental restructuring’ (adding objects to the environment: BCTTv1:12.5), that yielded consistent effect sizes when implemented. This particular deployment provided the most reliable relationship throughout the review; it was seen in all three experimental post-test studies, which provided earplugs within the environment, and all three measured a small to medium effect, for at least one recreational context. ‘Environmental restructuring’ should thus be considered for future recreational hearing protection interventions. Based on coding it would also be valuable to try other approaches in conjunction with environmental restructuring, such as incentivization (material incentive (behavior): BCTTv1.10.1), enablement (prompts and cues: BCTTv1:7.1) and modeling (salience of consequences: BCTTv1:5.2).

Studies indicate a lack of theory applied during the design process, or at least a lack of a description of theory within the methodologies. These issues have been raised by health psychologists previously, in that researchers face an uphill battle to replicate interventions to further test outcomes, [30, 55] due in part to poor reporting. Although one included paper applied the theory of planned behavior to design the evaluation questionnaires, [36] none of the included studies explicitly describes the use of theory for intervention design. The importance of behavior change theory and models (e.g., COM-B model) in hearing healthcare has been noted in the past, [28, 29] but appears still to be lacking within recreational hearing protection interventions. Therefore we would suggest better quality and more robust studies, achieved through use of theory and evidence, which will help target specific behavior change techniques and intervention functions to be incorporated in an effort to raise effect sizes. This use of theory driven practice will address gaps in knowledge in terms of quality and reporting for future systematic reviews, and help aid replication of interventions.

Limitations of this review include the inability to retrieve all data from the authors who were contacted directly. The provision of results that were missing or unclear in the original publications may have enabled a meta-analysis to be performed.

Conclusions

The present systematic review found very few hearing protection interventions addressing recreational noise exposure, a global hearing health concern. However, ‘environmental restructuring’ through the provision of earplugs (adding objects to the environment: BCTTv1:12.5), showed promise and might be considered a starting point for future interventions. Further hearing protection intervention studies should be conducted that employ randomized controlled designs, use systematic approaches to intervention development (e.g., the behavior change wheel [27]), consider intervention functions beyond education, such as incentivization (e.g., material incentive (behavior): BCTTv1.10.1), enablement (e.g., prompts and cues: BCTTv1:7.1) and modeling (e.g., salience of consequences: BCTTv1:5.2), and consider deploying previously unused behavior change techniques. Self-reported use of hearing protection has been widely used as a main outcome measure, but more objective assessments through observation or technological solutions would reduce the risk of reporting bias.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BCTTv1:

Behavior Change Technique Taxonomy Version 1

CASP:

Critical Appraisal Skills Program

COM-B:

Capability, Opportunity and Motivation model of Behavior

dBA:

A-weighted Decibel

DIY:

Do-it-yourself

MeSH:

Medical Subject Heading

References

  1. World Health Organization. Hearing loss due to recreational exposure to loud sounds: a review. World Health Organization; 2015. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/154589. Accessed November 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Guest H, Dewey RS, Plack CJ, Couth S, Prendergast G, Bakay W, Hall DA. The noise exposure structured interview (NESI): an instrument for the comprehensive estimation of lifetime noise exposure. Trends in hearing, 22. Article no: 2331216518803213. 2018. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/2331216518803213.

  3. Gopal K. Current practices in the assessment of recreational noise-induced hearing loss: a review: World Health Organization; 2017. https://www.who.int/pbd/deafness/Monograph_on_current_Practices_in_the_assessment_of_NIHL_in_recreational_settings.pdf?ua=1. Accessed November 2018.

  4. Washnik NJ, Phillips SL, Teglas S. Student’s music exposure: Full-day personal dose measurements. Noise Health. 2016;18(81):98–103. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4103/1463-1741.178510.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Dehnert K, Raab U, Perez-Alvarez C, Steffens T, Bolte G, Fromme H, Twardella D. Total leisure noise exposure and its association with hearing loss among adolescents. Int J Audiol. 2015;54(10):665–73. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/14992027.2015.1030510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Biassoni EC, Serra MR, Richter U, Joekes S, Yacci MR, Carignani JA, et al. Recreational noise exposure and its effects on the hearing of adolescents. Part II: development of hearing disorders. Int J Audiol. 2005;44(2):74–85. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/14992020500031728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gilliver M, Nguyen J, Beach EF, Barr C. Personal listening devices in Australia: Patterns of use and levels of risk. Semin Hear. 2017;38(4):282–97. .https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1055/s-0037-1606324.

  8. Lee D, Han W. Noise levels at baseball stadiums and the spectators’ attitude to noise. Noise Health. 2019;21(99):47–54. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4103/nah.NAH_39_18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Dolder CN, Suits JI, Wilson PS. Noise exposure in the general audience of a Formula 1 race. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics. 2013;20(1):040003. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1121/1.4865925.

  10. Opperman DA, Reifman W, Schlauch R, Levine S. Incidence of spontaneous hearing threshold shifts during modern concert performances. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2006;134(4):667–73. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.otohns.2005.11.039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Serra MR, Biassoni EC, Richter U, Minoldo G, Franco G, Abraham S, et al. Recreational noise exposure and its effects on the hearing of adolescents. Part I: an interdisciplinary long-term study. Int J Audiol. 2005;44(2):65–73. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/14992020400030010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bray A, Szymański M, Mills R. Noise induced hearing loss in dance music disc jockeys and an examination of sound levels in nightclubs. J Laryngol Otol. 2004;118(2):123–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1258/002221504772784577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lutman, M. E., Davis, A. C., & Ferguson, M. A. (2008). Epidemiological Evidence for the Effectiveness of the Noise at Work Regulations, RR669. https://www.hse.gov.uk/research/rrpdf/rr669.pdf. Accessed June 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  14. National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH). (1998). Criteria for a recommended standard: Occupational noise exposure. https://www.cdc.gov/niosh/docs/98-126/pdfs/98-126.pdf?id=10.26616/NIOSHPUB98126. Accessed 21 October 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Dalton DS, Cruickshanks KJ, Wiley TL, Klein BE, Klein R, Tweed TS. Association of Leisure-Time Noise Exposure and Hearing Loss: Asociación entre exposición a ruido durante el tiempo libre e hipoacusia. Audiology. 2001;40(1):1–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/00206090109073095.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Johnson O, Andrew B, Walker D, Morgan S, Aldren A. British university students' attitudes towards noise-induced hearing loss caused by nightclub attendance. J Laryngol Otol. 2014;128(1):29–34. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1017/S0022215113003241.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Degeest S, Corthals P, Vinck BM, Keppler H. Prevalence and characteristics of tinnitus after leisure noise exposure in young adults. Noise Health. 2014;16(68):26–33. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4103/1463-1741.127850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Ramakers GG, Kraaijenga VJ, Cattani G, van Zanten GA, Grolman W. Effectiveness of earplugs in preventing recreational noise–induced hearing loss: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Sur. 2016;142(6):551–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1001/jamaoto.2016.0225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Vogel I, Brug J, Van der Ploeg CP, Raat H. Prevention of adolescents’ music-induced hearing loss due to discotheque attendance: a Delphi study. Health Educ Res. 2009;24(6):1043–50. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/her/cyp031.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gupta N, Sharma A, Singh PP, Goyal A, Sao R. Assessment of knowledge of harmful effects and exposure to recreational music in college students of Delhi: a cross sectional exploratory study. Ind J Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2014;66(3):254–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s12070-013-0671-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Gilles A, Van Hal G, De Ridder D, Wouters K, Van de Heyning P. Epidemiology of noise-induced tinnitus and the attitudes and beliefs towards noise and hearing protection in adolescents. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e70297. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0070297.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. El Dib RP, Mathew JL, Martins RH. Interventions to promote the wearing of hearing protection. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012;4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD005234.pub5.

  23. Diviani N, Zanini C, Amann J, Rubinelli S. Make Listening Safe: Narrative Review on Listening Habits. World Health Organization; 2017. https://www.who.int/pbd/deafness/Momograph_on_Narrative_Overview_of_Systematic_Review_on_Listening_habits.pdf. Accessed 5th June 2019.

  24. Khan KM, Bielko SL, McCullagh MC. Efficacy of hearing conservation education programs for youth and young adults: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):1286. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-018-6198-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Serpanos YC, Berg AL, Renne B. Influence of hearing risk information on the motivation and modification of personal listening device use. Am J Audiol. 2016;25(4):332–43. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1044/2016_AJA-15-0062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Ferrari JR, Chan LM. INTERVENTIONS TO REDUCE HIGH-VOLUME PORTABLE HEADSETS:“TURN DOWN THE SOUND!”. J Appl Behav Anal. 1991;24(4):695–704. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-695.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Michie S, Atkins L, West R. 1st ed. the behaviour change wheel: a guide to designing interventions. London: Silverback Publishing; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Manchaiah VK. Health behavior change in hearing healthcare: a discussion paper. Audiol Res. 2012;2(1). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4081/audiores.2011.e4.

  29. Coulson NS, Ferguson MA, Henshaw H, Heffernan E. Applying theories of health behaviour and change to hearing health research: time for a new approach. International Journal of Audiology, 55(sup3), S99-S104. 2016. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/14992027.2016.1161851.

  30. Michie S, Richardson M, Johnston M, Abraham C, Francis J, Hardeman W, Eccles MP, Cane J, Wood CE. The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann Behav Med. 2013;46(1):81–95. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Howlett N, Trivedi D, Troop NA, Chater AM. Are physical activity interventions for healthy inactive adults effective in promoting behavior change and maintenance, and which behavior change techniques are effective? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl Behav Med. 2019;9(1):147–57. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/tbm/iby010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Bondaronek P, Alkhaldi G, Slee A, Hamilton FL, Murray E. Quality of publicly available physical activity apps: review and content analysis. JMIR mHealth uHealth. 2018;6(3):e53. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2196/mhealth.9069.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JP, Clarke M, Devereaux PJ, Kleijnen J, Moher D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000100. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Beach EF, Nielsen L, Gilliver M. Providing earplugs to young adults at risk encourages protective behaviour in music venues. Glob Health Promot. 2016;23(2):45–56. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1757975914558887.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Cha J, Smukler SR, Chung Y, House R, Bogoch II. Increase in use of protective earplugs by rock and roll concert attendees when provided for free at concert venues. Int J Audiol. 2015;54(12):984–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/14992027.2015.1080863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Gilles A, Van de Heyning P. Effectiveness of a preventive campaign for noise-induced hearing damage in adolescents. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2014;78(4):604–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.ijporl.2014.01.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Keppler H, Ingeborg D, Sofie D, Bart V. The effects of a hearing education program on recreational noise exposure, attitudes and beliefs toward noise, hearing loss, and hearing protector devices in young adults. Noise Health. 2015;17(78):253–62. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4103/1463-1741.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Marlenga B, Linneman JG, Pickett W, Wood DJ, Kirkhorn SR, Broste SK, Knobloch MJ, Berg RL. Randomized trial of a hearing conservation intervention for rural students: long-term outcomes. Pediatrics. 2011;128(5):e1139–46. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1542/peds.2011-0770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Neyen S. Acceptance concerning sound level limits with pupils in the age between 10 and 19 years. Zeitschrift fur Larmbekampfung. 2003;50(2):54–62.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Weichbold V, Zorowka P. Effects of a hearing protection campaign on the discotheque attendance habits of high-school students: Efectos de una campaña de protección auditiva en los hábitos de asistencia a discotecas de estudiantes de educación media. Int J Audiol. 2003;42(8):489–93. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/14992020309081519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Weichbold V, Zorowka P. Can a hearing education campaign for adolescents change their music listening behavior? ¿ Puede una campaña de educación auditiva para adolescentes cambiar sus conductas para escuchar música? Int J Audiol. 2007;46(3):128–33. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/14992020601126849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Higgins JP, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions Version 5.1. 0 [updated March 2011]. 2011. The Cochrane collaboration. 2019 Feb.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Critical Appraisal Skills Program. CASP checklist: randomised controlled trials. Critical Appraisal Skills Program; 2019. https://casp-uk.net/wp-content/uploads/2018/01/CASP-Randomised-Controlled-Trial-Checklist-2018.pdf. Accessed February 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Critical Appraisal Skills Program. CASP checklist: cohort study. Critical Appraisal Skills Program; 2019. https://casp-uk.net/wp-content/uploads/2018/01/CASP-Cohort-Study-Checklist_2018.pdf. Accessed February 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Lenhard W, Lenhard A. Calculation of Effect Sizes. Dettelbach: Psychometrica; 2016. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.13140/RG.2.1.3478.4245. https://www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html. Accessed October 2018.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  46. Behaviour Change Technique Taxonomy. Taxonomy training: online training. Behaviour Change Technique Taxonomy; 2019. http://www.bct-taxonomy.com/. Accessed February 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Balanay JA, Kearney GD. Attitudes toward noise, perceived hearing symptoms, and reported use of hearing protection among college students: influence of youth culture. Noise Health. 2015;17(79):394–405. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4103/1463-1741.169701.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. McCombe AW, Binnington J, Nash D. Two solutions to the problem of noise exposure for motorcyclists. Occup Med. 1994;44(5):239–42. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/occmed/44.5.239.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Knobel KA, Lima MC. Effectiveness of the Brazilian version of the dangerous decibels® educational program. Int J Audiol. 2014;53(sup2):S35–42. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/14992027.2013.857794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Griest SE, Folmer RL, Martin WH. Effectiveness of “Dangerous Decibels,” a school-based hearing loss prevention program. Am J Audiol. 2007;16(2):S165–81. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1044/1059-0889(2007/021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Chermak GD, Curtis L, Seikel JA. The effectiveness of an interactive hearing conservation program for elementary school children. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 1996;27(1):29–39. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1044/0161-1461.2701.29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Chermak GD, Peters-McCarthy E. The effectiveness of an educational hearing conservation program for elementary school children. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 1991;22(1):308–12. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1044/0161-1461.2201.308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Sheeran P, Webb TL. The intention–behavior gap. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2016;10(9):503–18. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/spc3.12265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Meinke DK, Finan DS, Flamme GA, Murphy WJ, Stewart M, Lankford JE, Tasko S. (2017). Prevention of noise-induced hearing loss from recreational firearms. In Seminars in hearing, 38(04):267–281. Thieme Medical Publishers. doi:https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1055/s-0037-1606323.

  55. Michie S, Johnston M. Theories and techniques of behaviour change: developing a cumulative science of behaviour change. Health Psychol Rev. 2012;6(1):1–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/17437199.2012.654964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the NIHR Manchester Biomedical Research Centre and supported by the NIHR Greater Manchester Patient Safety Translational Research Centre. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Funding

This research was funded by the NIHR Manchester Biomedical Research Centre with support from the NIHR Greater Manchester Patient Safety Translational Research Centre. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The funders had no role in study design, collection of data, analysis, interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to data and had final responsibility to submit for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ML and CJA conceived the project, designed the study, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. ML carried out database searches, screened titles, abstracts and full texts, extracted data from citations, prepared for analyses, carried out analyses and scored quality. CJA provided full supervision to ML throughout all stages. SL provided assistance with screening titles, abstracts and full texts, data extraction and analyses. SL scored quality and helped resolve conflicts. CJA helped resolve conflicts for inclusion of articles, data extraction and data analyses. SL and CP helped prepare the final manuscript. CP provided secondary supervision to ML. ML and CJA critically revised manuscript at all times. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael T. Loughran.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

There are no conflicts of interest to report.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

Search Strategy Supplement.

Additional file 2.

Supplement Table.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Loughran, M.T., Lyons, S., Plack, C.J. et al. Which interventions increase hearing protection behaviors during noisy recreational activities? A systematic review. BMC Public Health 20, 1376 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-020-09414-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-020-09414-w

Keywords