Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Infancy weight gain, parental socioeconomic position, and childhood overweight and obesity: a Danish register-based cohort study

Abstract

Background

Rapid infant weight gain (RIWG) is a very strong predictor of childhood overweight and obesity (COO). Socioeconomic position (SEP) is also related to the risk of COO and parents of different SEP may differ in their reaction to accelerated infant weight gain. Together this could lead to differences in how weight gain and COO risk relate across SEP. This study aimed to analyse possible interaction of SEP and RIWG on COO risk.

Methods

A register-based longitudinal cohort study followed 19,894 healthy, term infants, born in Denmark between December 2011 and May 2015. Logistic regression models were used to estimate odds ratios (OR) of COO risk at 2 years (22–26 months) of age with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for categories of infancy weight gain based on changes in weight-for-age z-scores between 0 and 8–10 months of age (slow (<− 0.67), mean (− 0.67–0.67), rapid (> 0.67–1.34) and very rapid (> 1.34)). Possible multiplicative and additive interaction of SEP (based on household income and maternal education) on the relationship between infancy weight gain and COO were analysed.

Results

In total, 19.1 and 15.1% experienced rapid or very rapid weight gain, respectively, and 1497 (7.5%) children were classified with COO at follow-up. These prevalences were higher in those with lower levels of SEP. Adjusted OR for COO were 3.09 (95% CI [2.66–3.59]) and 7.58 (95% CI [6.51–8.83]) for rapid and very rapid weight gain, respectively, when household income was included in the model. Results were similar in the model including maternal education. No signs of interactions were detected on a multiplicative scale. Weak signs of additive interaction were present, but these values did not reach significance.

Conclusion

Both rapid and very rapid weight gain were associated with substantially higher risks of COO but these associations were not modified by SEP. This indicates that promotion of healthy weight gain should take place in all population groups irrespective of their SEP.

Peer Review reports

Background

Rapid infant weight gain (RIWG), generally defined as a change of more than 0.67 standard deviations in weight-for-age z-score between two time-points during infancy, is strongly and consistently related to overweight and obesity later in life [1,2,3,4]. A recent meta-analysis found that RIWG was associated with an increased risk of both childhood and adulthood overweight and obesity (pooled odds ratio, 3.66 (95% CI [2.59 to 5.17]) in comparison with those not experiencing RIWG [4]. Although the causal pathways are not clearly known, it has been proposed that infants with RIWG have a higher level of insulin resistance, central fat deposition and general fat accumulation that increase their risk of childhood overweight and obesity (COO) [4,5,6,7,8]. RIWG is one of the first measurable indications of a growth trajectory that could lead to later obesity [9], which makes prevention of this weight gain pattern during infancy an important target in an early-life obesity prevention strategy.

Higher prevalence of RIWG is reported in populations of low socioeconomic position (SEP) [10, 11], and the relationship between RIWG and COO also appears stronger in observational studies comprising of populations with high proportions of low SEP individuals [12]. Socioeconomic position represents access to resources like money, power, knowledge, and social support [13], that can be used to avoid exposure to, or help to buffer the negative effects of, general risk factors and stressors [14]. In a complex interplay together with individual, social and environmental factors, these macro-levelled factors are of significance for shaping healthy behaviour [15], for instance in relation to infant feeding [16].

Short duration of breastfeeding [17], formula feeding [18, 19], early weaning [17, 20], feeding on a schedule [19], feeding using large bottles [21], and feeding high protein formula [22] are factors associated with RIWG, which typically are more commonly practised in groups of low SEP [23] and could thus help to explain the suggested stronger association between RIWG and COO in low SEP populations. Another potential explanation is social differences in views on parenting. Qualitative studies on American low-income populations [24, 25] have found that families emphasize heavy infants as indicators of good infant health and successful parenting and a strong need to be reassured that infants are provided with enough nutrition. Such views and beliefs, in addition to a potentially higher level of concerns and stressors in low SEP parents, could encourage the use of weight gain-enhancing feeding strategies to achieve infant weight gain and further accelerate infant weight gain leading to a higher risk of COO. Qualitative differences in the response for children with RIWG, in addition to socioeconomic differences in parental identification of infant overweight or obesity [26, 27], in social support for breastfeeding [28], in early weaning [29], or in the usage of these strategies to promote and prolong infant sleep [25, 30], could potentially have implications for how parents notice and respond to a rapid infancy weight gain trajectory and lead to effect modification of SEP on the association by RIWG on COO.

Several other pre-and postnatal risk factors for COO are furthermore socially patterned [23], and some, e.g. maternal pre-pregnancy obesity [31, 32], smoking during pregnancy [33,34,35], and occurrence of gestational diabetes [36], are proposed to cause pre-programming effects that increase the risk of both postnatal accelerated weight gain and subsequent obesity. The effect on COO risk can be amplified if several biological and social risk factors cluster and interact simultaneously, which may be the case for people with low SEP [37].

Overall, this provide a basis for evaluating whether the relationship between infancy weight gain and COO risk differs across different levels of SEP in order to identify potential high-risk groups in need of more attention by early-life obesity prevention strategies. Few studies have comprehensively tested whether the relationship between RIWG and COO risk is modified by SEP. Two studies did not identify any effect modification by maternal education [38, 39], but these studies had sample sizes around 300 and their null findings may represent a type II error. The aim of this study is therefore to analyse the relationship between RIWG and COO risk across different levels of SEP in a large cohort. We hypothesised that the association between rapid infant weight gain and risk of developing COO would be stronger for infants from parents with low SEP than for infants from parents with high SEP.

Methods

Data sources

This register-based longitudinal cohort study links The Children’s Database with other nationwide administrative Danish registries. The administrative register The Children’s Database has collected data on children’s (0–17 years) health status since 2009. Data is compiled at regular preventive health assessments during infancy and childhood [40, 41]. From December 2011 it has been mandatory for Danish municipalities to report data collected by health visitors, including data on child height and weight [42]. Every Danish citizen is given a unique Civil Personal Register number at birth, which enables linkage of individual-levelled data across nationwide administrative registries [43, 44]. Individual-levelled data for the study population and their parents were obtained from The Medical Birth Registry [45], The Danish Civil Registration System [44], The Income Statistics Register [46], and The Population Education Register [47].

Population

The study population consisted of children born in Denmark between December 2011 and May 2015 and registered in The Children’s Database. Eligible infants were born at term (gestational age of 37 + 0 to 41 + 6) with birthweight > 2500 g and were registered with both their 8-, 9- or 10-months weight and their 22-, 23-, 24-, 25- or 26-months weight and height.

Child anthropometric measures

Health professionals measured weight and height during planned health examinations. Weight was measured using a validated scale and reported to the nearest 0.1 kg. Height was measured recumbent until two years of age and reported to the nearest 0.5 cm [40]. We calculated weight-for-age and body mass index z-scores describing infants anthropometric standard deviations in relation to the 2006 World Health Organization (WHO) Child Growth Standards using WHO Anthro 2011, version 3.2.2 [48]. Infant weight gain was defined as the change in weight-for-age z-scores between birth and 8 to 10 months. A change of 0.67 represents moving one percentile band on a growth chart [3, 4, 49], and the change in weight gain were thus categorised as: <− 0.67 standard deviations (SD) (slow weight gain), − 0.67 to 0.67 SD (mean weight gain), > 0.67 to 1.34 SD (rapid weight gain), and > 1.34 SD (very rapid weight gain). The timing of measurement was chosen as RIWG during the first, compared to the second, year of life has shown a stronger association with later risk of overweight and obesity [4] and because a preventive health assessments including infant measurement is offered around 8–10 months of age in Denmark. The measurement closest to 9 months of age were used if several measurements were available. Implausible values (<− 6 or > 5 SD) were excluded [50]. The study outcome was childhood overweight or obesity at 2 years of age, which was defined as having a body mass index z-score of more than 2 SD [51, 52]. The measurements closest to 24 months of age were applied if several measurements were available, and implausible values (<− 5 or > 5 SD) were excluded [50].

Socioeconomic position

Maternal education and household income served as proxies for the socioeconomic position. Level of maternal education closest to the year before or after birth was identified through The Population Education Register. This information was categorised in accordance with the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2011 [53] and adjusted to a Danish education setting as done by Ullits and colleagues [54], thus consisting of the four categories: ISCED level 0–2 (up to and including lower secondary education), 4 (post-secondary non-tertiary education), 5–6 (short-cycle tertiary education, or Bachelor’s degree or equivalent) and 7–8 (Master’s/Doctoral degree or equivalent). Household income level was identified through The Income Statistics Register and defined as mean annual household income calculated from the two years prior to birth and divided into quartiles (low, low-middle, high-middle and high).

Covariates

A recent review on the relationship between RIWG and COO informed the selection of relevant child and maternal covariates [4]; child sex, breastfeeding duration, birthweight, mode of delivery, maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI), maternal smoking status during pregnancy, gestational age at birth, parity, and gestational diabetes status. Data on infant sex and breastfeeding duration were identified from The Children’s Database. WHO recommend six months of exclusive breastfeeding [55], but many parents introduce complementary foods from four months of age [56]. Breastfeeding duration was thus categorised into four groups: 0–2 months, 2–4 months, 4–6 months, > 6 months, where the cut-off at two months was set to separate short- and medium-termed breastfeeding. Information on infant birthweight, mode of delivery, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, smoking status during pregnancy, gestational age at birth, parity and occurrence of gestational diabetes were identified in The Medical Birth Registry. Birthweight and gestational age at birth were applied to identify infant size for gestational age. Thresholds reported by Kramer and colleagues [57] were applied to define three categories: Small- (SGA), appropriate- (AGA) and large-for-gestational age (LGA). Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was grouped in accordance with WHO categories: < 18.5 (underweight), 18.5–24.9 (normal weight), 25.0–29.9 (overweight) 30.0–34.9 (obesity class I) and > 35.0 (obesity class II + III) [58]. Information on maternal smoking status during pregnancy was divided into three groups: Yes, Stopped during pregnancy, No. Gestational age at birth was included as the week of gestation, and parity was categorised as 1, 2 or > 2 completed pregnancies. Mode of delivery consisted of the categories vaginal birth and caesarean section. Finally, information on infant’s birth year, region of habitation at the time of the last anthropometric measurement, and the number of anthropometric measurements registered in The Children’s Database was included in all adjusted models in order to control for potential differences.

Statistical analysis

Between-group differences in covariate distribution across exposure categories (infancy weight gain, maternal education level and household income level) were tested using Chi-Square (X2) tests. Missing values were imputed by chained equations using the mice package in R [59]. The number of imputations was set to 33 and reflected the highest percentage of missing information on a single covariate, with 20 iterations in each chain [59]. All covariates in addition to categorical information on maternal age, birth length, and maternal ethnicity were used to inform the imputation. These additional covariates were left out of the main analyses due to the risk of over-adjustment. The administrative databases lack information on educational attainment and household income for some citizens, especially for immigrants that had their education before entering Denmark. Observations with missing data on educational attainment and income (n = 1123) were therefore excluded prior to imputation due to a potential violation of the missing not at random assumption [60]. Complete case analyses were carried out as sensitivity analyses. Furthermore, the study population and the population excluded due to missing anthropometric data were compared on sex, size for gestational age at birth, maternal education, household income, weight gain category, and COO.

Unadjusted, intermediate partly adjusted, and fully adjusted logistic regression model were used to estimate odds ratios (OR) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CI) of COO risks. Two intermediate models comprised of either maternal education or income level, while two fully adjusted models additionally included all covariates. This approach was chosen, as we wanted to examine the potential confounding effect of SEP alone if SEP was not found to be an effect modifier in the relationship between weight gain category and COO. Multiplicative interaction between weight gain category and SEP were tested in the fully adjusted models. Models including all covariates were stratified for SEP to further study between-group differences. Further search for high-risk groups in special need for preventive attention was conducted via tests for additive interaction between weight gain category and SEP. This was evaluated through the calculation of relative excess risk due to interaction (RERI) using the method involving categorical exposures, as described by VanderWeele and Knol [61]. RERI was calculated based on 2 × 2 tables. Maternal education and household income were dichotomised in order to ease interpretations of the results. Dichotomisation involved collapsing the two lowest and two highest categories. Comparisons between the categories of mean weight gain and rapid or very rapid weight gain were carried out. This enabled calculation of RERI for four subgroups. High level of SEP and mean weight gain category were coded as reference, as these exposure groups produced the lowest outcome risk when considered jointly [62]. These analyses were conducted on complete case data, as the method described was not developed for application on imputed data. Data management and calculation of RERI was conducted using the SAS Statistical Software package for Windows, version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA), and all further statistical analyses were conducted by using the R statistical software, version 3.5.0 [63].

Ethics

This study was approved by The Danish Data Protection Agency (ref: 2008-58-0028, internal ref.: 2017–67). In Denmark, it is not necessary to obtain ethical approval for the use of administrative register information in research, as long as the project does not involve human biological material [64].

Results

Study population

In total, 196,376 infants born between December 2011 and May 2015 were registered at least once with their height or weight in The Children’s Database. Figure 1 illustrates how the study population of 19,894 children was arrived at. Their background information stratified by weight gain category is shown in Table 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart illustrating the process of selecting the study population. Flow chart showing how the study population was arrived at and at what time the applied measurements were collected

Table 1 Table of covariate distribution on category of infancy weight gain

Of the study population, 3800 (19.1%) were classified with rapid and 2998 (15.1%) with very rapid weight gain. The distribution of all covariates except maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and occurrence of gestational diabetes differed statistical significantly across weight gain categories. Compared to infants with mean weight gain, a higher proportion of infants with rapid or very rapid weight gain were born SGA, before 39 weeks of gestation, by caesarean section, by primiparous mothers or by mothers that smoked during pregnancy, and were breastfed for less than 2 months (Table 1). The prevalence of rapid and very rapid weight gain increased with decreasing level of maternal education and household income. A social gradient was evident for rapid and very rapid weight gain when measured by either the level of maternal education or household income. Covariate distribution appeared mostly similar in both maternal education and household income (Additional files 1 and 2). The lowest prevalences of most risk factors were seen in the highest positioned groups.

Risk of COO on infant weight gain category

A total of 1497 (7.5%) individuals were categorised with COO at follow-up at two years of age (22–26 months). The proportion increased with increasing weight gain category. 932 (15.9%) of the 5866 infants with rapid or very rapid weight gain were classified with COO. In the unadjusted model, the risk of COO was increased in infants with rapid (OR 2.18, [95%CI 1.90 to 2.51]) and very rapid weight gain (OR 4.10, [95%CI 3.59 to 4.68]) when compared to infants with mean weight gain (Table 2). These estimates remained unchanged after adjusting for SEP in the intermediate models, but they increased considerably after adjusting for covariates (Adjusted model 2a and 2b, Table 2). The risk of COO for each specific infancy weight gain category were relatively similar across levels of maternal education and household income in the stratified analyses, and confidence intervals for each specific infancy weight gain category overlapped when compared across SEP categories (Table 3). Correspondingly, no signs of interactions on a multiplicative scale were detected (maternal education: p = 0.89, household income: p = 0.24).

Table 2 Risk of childhood overweight and obesity calculated from logistic regression models
Table 3 Childhood overweight and obesity risk, stratified by level of socioeconomic position

Relative excess risk due to interaction for weight gain category and SEP

The values of RERI were generally positive, indicating super-additive effect modification when comparing the combined effect of low SEP and rapid or very rapid weight gain on COO risk (Table 4). Only the RERI for household income and rapid weight gain were negative (RERI: -0.19 [95%CI − 1.19 to 0.81]), but the combination of household income level and very rapid weight gain produced the largest positive RERI (RERI: 1.07 [95%CI − 0.97 to 3.11]). Although indicating super-additive effect modification in most combinations, none of these reached statistical significance.

Table 4 Analyses of additive interaction

Sensitivity analysis

Of the study population, 13,157 (66.1%) had complete information on all covariates. Frequencies of missing data were generally low except 32.7% lacking information on breastfeeding duration. Main results from the complete case analyses did not differ notably from results based on imputed data (Additional files 3 and 4). The distribution of weight gain category and COO in infants excluded due to missing anthropometric information did not differ from the distribution in the study population (Additional file 5). Significant differences were observed in the distribution of infant sex, size for gestational age at birth, maternal education and household income when comparing infants with and without missing data on exposure and outcome.

Discussion

Key results and interpretation

Our results suggest that the relationship between rapid and very rapid infancy weight gain and COO are consistent across socioeconomic positions, implying that SEP does not modify this risk-relationship. There were weak signs of additive interaction in some combinations of SEP and rapid or very rapid weight gain on COO risk, suggesting that the public health value of preventing rapid or very rapid weight gain as a part of an early-life COO prevention strategy could be greater if carried out in groups of low compared to high SEP groups. However, these results did not reach significance. Thus, there were no support for our hypotheses proposing that the association between rapid infant weight gain and risk of developing COO were stronger in infants from parents with low than with high SEP. Thus, our comprehensive register-based cohort study confirms the results of the previous, smaller studies within this area [38, 39], and our findings suggest that activities involving prevention of RIWG and promotion of healthy infant weight gain as a part of an early-life COO prevention strategy can be relevant for all socioeconomic groups.

As in previous studies [10, 11], our results show a social gradient in infancy weight gain, where the prevalence of rapid and very rapid weight gain is higher in those with lower levels of SEP. The gradient was clearest in infants with very rapid weight gain, and higher accumulation of risk factors in infants with this weight gain pattern as well as in infants from low SEP could help to explain the social gradient. Similarly, the risk of COO increased with decreasing SEP in the unadjusted analyses. Much of this social gradient disappeared after adjusting for covariates, which indicate that a social distribution of the pre- and postnatal risk factors accounted for in this study could explain these social differences to some extent. However, risk estimates were larger for household income than for maternal eduction levels, and only the estimate for the group with the lowest income level remained significant after adjusting for covariates. This may suggest that processes linking income to health can be of special importance for early life COO risk [65]. Better access to quality food, education, health and leisure services may then be central mechanisms [65, 66].

The prevalence of COO risk factors like being born LGA, having high maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and maternal smoking during pregnancy were unevenly distributed across weight gain categories in the study population. We found that the association between infancy weight gain and COO more than doubled for both rapid and very rapid weight gain after adjusting for covariates, which suggests that other factors confounded this relationship. Our study focus was limited to considering effect modification between infancy weight gain and SEP on COO risk, and no further analyses were carried out to identify potentially responsible covariate(s). Lamb et al. [36] found that the relationship between size for gestational age and higher childhood BMI was strengthened after adjusting for infancy weight gain during the first year of life, and they explained this by suggesting that high birthweight infants still have increased risk of overweight and obesity in later childhood despite having slower weight gain trajectories during the first year of life. Notably, the risk of COO also increased considerably for LGA infants in the fully adjusted models, and this might give some support to the hypothesis proposing that the risk of COO may already commence during pregnancy through pre-programming processes that promote high birthweight [67, 68].

In the study population, a total of 15.1% experienced very rapid weight gain, and this weight gain category was associated with a seven-fold increase in COO risk when compared to infants with mean weight gain. This is consistent with the findings of a meta-analysis by Druet et al. [69], where very rapid weight gain during the first year of life, which is the equivalent to moving up two or more percentiles on a growth chart, was a particularly important predictor for later COO development. The implications of very rapid weight gain on COO risk can also be traced longer-termed, as Johnson et al. [70] observed that infants with very rapid weight gain between 0 and 3 years of age had higher mean BMI and COO risks at the ages of 11 and 14 years. This increase was higher than the increase observed in children with rapid weight gain only.

Strengths and limitations

RIWG has commonly been studied as a dichotomization using a cut-off score of > 0.67 SD change in weight, as suggested in the seminal paper by Ong in 2000 [49]. As opposed to previous studies [38, 39], the large sample in the current study enabled a more detailed elaboration and division between rapid and very rapid weight gain in relation to population characteristics and COO risk. Our results showing significant risk differences between weight gain categories suggest that application of a simpler weight gain categorisation could result in a notable information loss. A more nuanced categorization may improve interpretation of risk estimates, as the group of reference simply consist of infants with mean weight gain rather than a combination of both slow and mean weight gain.

Another strength is having up-to-date information from national administrative databases on both infants and parents, thus enabling comprehensive confounder adjustment in multivariable regression models on contemporary data. The choice of covariates was informed by literature, but it is possible that our models only partly capture the true relationship between infancy weight gain, socioeconomic status and childhood obesity, as this relationship is fairly complex [71]. We could have conducted a more comprehensive conceptual interpretation of the relationship in e.g. a directed acyclic graph (DAG), but the traditional approach was chosen as this relationship is complicated and would possibly not be captured in a DAG.

Using data not collected for the purpose of this study can also have introduced some limitations. General practitioners are, in contrast to the health visitors, not legally obliged to register their data to the database, so local differences in data registration over time may have occurred. Information on the number of registrations for each child, birth year and administrative region were therefore included in the adjusted models in order to reduce the magnitude of this potential selection bias.

Furthermore, a high proportion of missing data was observed for the duration of breastfeeding. The task of collecting and reporting these data to The Children’s Database is generally new to health visitors, and the lack of everyday routines could have compromised data quality and completeness. However, we have no reason to believe that the data quality is compromised differently across exposure or outcome categories and possible bias would thus lead towards the null and not explain the large OR detected in our study. Furthermore, multiple imputations were made on missing values and results from complete case analyses support the robustness of our main results. We assumed that missingness of breastfeeding data were related to regional differences in registrations or birth year rather than to the length of breastfeeding itself, and multiple imputation are a more efficient method associated with less bias than complete case analyses when data are missing at random [72]. The validity of the additive interaction analyses can be questioned as these were based on complete case data, but the consistency in analyses made on both imputed and complete case data support the validity of these results.

A large proportion of infants were not registered with sufficient data in order to define exposure and outcome, why our results could be affected by selection bias. However, no significant difference was observed in the distribution of exposure or outcome when comparing populations with and without missing data, which suggest that potential effect of such bias on the results may be small. Similarly, we do not believe that differences in covariate distribution in these populations have had any significant impact on the direction of the results, as the distribution differences were small and reached significance presumably due to high power.

Finally, the generalisability of our findings is restricted to populations born at term with a birthweight of more than 2500 g. Postnatal growth of infants experiencing in-utero growth restrictions may differ from other infants due to physiological mechanisms [5], why our study population did not comprise low birthweight infants. Low birthweight is more common in groups of low SEP [23], and exclusion of low birthweight infants could thus have led to greater consistency in the relationship between birthweight, weight gain and COO across different SEP groups. It is possible that our results would appear differently if we had included low birthweight infants, and this should be assessed in future studies.

Implications

Weak signs of additive interaction in some combinations of SEP and rapid or very rapid weight gain on COO risk were identified, but the results as a whole does show any signs of COO risk increase in any SEP groups after rapid or very rapid weight gain. Thus, there does not currently exist suffiecient evidence to suggest that promotion of healthy infant weight gain as a part of an early-life COO prevention strategy should be targeted certain socioeconomic groups, but future research should pursue the signs of additive interaction identified in our study. Anyhow, the social gradient in the prevalence of rapid and very rapid weight gain highlights that such prevention should make use of approaches that are effective in populations of low SEP. Access to resources such as money, knowledge, and power affects individuals’ ability to achieve behaviour change, and a lack of access to these resources can make individual focused behaviour change interventions less successful in populations of low SEP compared to those of high SEP [13, 73]. Conversely, interventions that integrate an intend to make changes at more upstream levels like the community, institutional, or policy level may be better strategic choices, as such interventions are more successful in reducing social inequalities in health [74, 75].

Conclusion

Over one-third of the study population experienced rapid (> 0.67 SD) or very rapid (> 1.34 SD) weight gain within the first year of life and 7.5% were categorised with COO at 2 years of age in this Danish register-based cohort study. A social gradient was observed in both the prevalence of these weight gain patterns and the risk of COO. The risk of experiencing COO was over seven times higher for infants with very rapid compared to mean weight gain after adjusting for confounders, but there were no clear signs of modification by SEP on the relationship between rapid and very rapid weight gain category and COO risk. This suggest that prevention of RIWG and promotion of healthy infancy weight gain as a strategy for early-life obesity prevention will be valuable for all socioeconomic groups. Such prevention should make use of strategies that are effective in low SEP groups, as the prevalence of rapid and very rapid weight gain were higher in these groups.

Availability of data and materials

Due to Danish law and protecting patient privacy, the combined set of data used in this study can only be made available through a trusted third party, Statistics Denmark. This state organisation holds the data used for this study. University-based Danish scientific organisations can be authorized to work with data within Statistics Denmark and such organisation can provide access to individual scientists. Requests for data may be sent to Statistics Denmark: http://www.dst.dk/en/OmDS/organisation/TelefonbogOrg.aspx?kontor=13&tlfbogsort=sektion or the Danish Data Protection Agency: https://www.datatilsynet.dk/english/the-danish-data-protection-agency/contact/.

Abbreviations

AGA:

Appropriate-for-Gestational Age

BMI:

Body mass index

CI:

Confidence interval

DAG:

Directed acyclic graph

ISCED:

International Standard Classification of Education

LGA:

Large-for-Gestational Age

OR:

Odds ratio

RERI:

Relative excess risk due to interaction

RIWG:

Rapid infant weight gain

COO:

Childhood overweight and obesity

SD:

Standard deviation

SEP:

Socioeconomic position

SGA:

Small-for-Gestational Age

WHO:

World Health Organization

References

  1. Weng SF, Redsell SA, Swift JA, Yang M, Glazebrook CP. Systematic review and meta-analyses of risk factors for childhood overweight identifiable during infancy. Arch Dis Child. 2012 Dec;97(12):1019–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Woo Baidal JA, Locks LM, Cheng ER, Blake-Lamb TL, Perkins ME, Taveras EM. Risk factors for childhood obesity in the first 1,000 days. Am J Prev Med. 2016;50(6):761–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Monteiro POA, Victora CG. Rapid growth in infancy and childhood and obesity in later life – a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2005 May;6(2):143–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Zheng M, Lamb KE, Grimes C, Laws R, Bolton K, Ong KK, et al. Rapid weight gain during infancy and subsequent adiposity: a systematic review and meta-analysis of evidence. Obes Rev. 2017;19(3):321–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Brands B, Demmelmair H, Koletzko B. How growth due to infant nutrition influences obesity and later disease risk. Acta Paediatr. 2014;103(6):578–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Stettler N. Nature and strength of epidemiological evidence for origins of childhood and adulthood obesity in the first year of life. Int J Obes. 2007;31(7):1035–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Durmuş B, Mook-Kanamori DO, Holzhauer S, Hofman A, van der Beek EM, Boehm G, et al. Growth in foetal life and infancy is associated with abdominal adiposity at the age of 2 years: the generation R study. Clin Endocrinol. 2010;72(5):633–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Ong KK, Emmett P, Northstone K, Golding J, Rogers I, Ness AR, et al. Infancy weight gain predicts childhood body fat and age at menarche in girls. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2009;94(5):1527–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Elks CE, Loos RJF, Sharp SJ, Langenberg C, Ring SM, Timpson NJ, et al. Genetic markers of adult obesity risk are associated with greater early infancy weight gain and growth. PLoS Med. 2010;7(5):e1000284.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Wijlaars LPMM, Johnson L, van Jaarsveld CHM, Wardle J. Socioeconomic status and weight gain in early infancy. Int J Obes. 2011;35(7):963–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Reeske A, Spallek J, Bammann K, Eiben G, De Henauw S, Kourides Y, et al. Migrant background and eeight gain in early infancy: results from the German study sample of the IDEFICS study. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e60648.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Andrea SB, Hooker ER, Messer LC, Tandy T, Boone-Heinonen J. Does the association between early life growth and later obesity differ by race/ethnicity or socioeconomic status? A systematic review. Ann Epidemiol. 2017;27(9):583–92 e5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Link BG, Phelan JO. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Am Sociol. 1995;(Extra Issue):80–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Elstad JI. The psycho-social perspective on social inequalities in health. Sociol Health Illn. 1998;20(5):598–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Sallis JF, Owen N. Ecological models of health behaviour. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health behavior: Theory, research, and practice. Elektronis. San Francisco: San Francisco, California: Jossey-Bass; 2015. p. 43–65.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Boak R, Virgo-Milton M, Hoare A, de Silva A, Gibbs L, Gold L, et al. Choosing foods for infants: a qualitative study of the factors that influence mothers. Child Care Health Dev. 2016;42(3):359–69.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Chrestani MA, Santos IS, Horta BL, Dumith SC, De Oliveira Dode MAS. Associated factors for accelerated growth in childhood: a systematic review. Matern Child Health J. 2013;17(3):512–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Li R, Magadia J, Fein SB, Grummer-Strawn LM. Risk of bottle-feeding for rapid weight gain during the first year of life. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(5):431–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Mihrshahi S, Battistutta D, Magarey A, Daniels LA. Determinants of rapid weight gain during infancy: baseline results from the NOURISH randomised controlled trial. BMC Pediatr. 2011;11:99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Sloan S, Gildea A, Stewart M, Sneddon H, Iwaniec D. Early weaning is related to weight and rate of weight gain in infancy. Child Care Health Dev. 2008;34(1):59–64.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Wood CT, Skinner AC, Yin HS, Rothman RL, Sanders LM, Delamater AM, et al. Bottle size and weight gain in formula-fed infants. Pediatrics. 2016;138(1):e20154538.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Weber M, Grote V, Closa-Monasterolo R, Escribano J, Langhendries J-P, Dain E, et al. Lower protein content in infant formula reduces BMI and obesity risk at school age: follow-up of a randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(5):1041–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Cameron AJ, Spence AC, Laws R, Hesketh KD, Lioret S, Campbell KJ. A review of the relationship between socioeconomic position and the early-life predictors of obesity. Curr Obes Rep. 2015;4(3):350–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Baughcum AE, Burklow KA, Deeks CM, Powers SW, Whitaker RC. Maternal feeding practices and childhood obesity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1998;152(10):1010–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Heinig MJ, Follett JR, Ishii KD, Kavanagh-Prochaska K, Cohen R, Panchula J. Barriers to compliance with infant-feeding recommendations among low-income women. J Hum Lact. 2006;22(1):27–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Baughcum AE, Chamberlin LA, Deeks CM, Powers SW, Whitaker RC. Maternal perceptions of overweight preschool children. Pediatrics. 2000;106(6):1380–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Genovesi S, Giussani M, Faini A, Vigorita F, Pieruzzi F, Strepparava MG, et al. Maternal perception of excess weight in children: a survey conducted by paediatricians in the province of Milan. Acta Paediatr Int J Paediatr. 2005;94(6):747–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Mitra AK, Khoury AJ, Hinton AW, Carothers C. Predictors of breastfeeding intention among low-income women. Matern Child Health J. 2004;8(2):65–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Scott JA, Binns CW, Graham KI, Oddy WH. Predictors of the early introduction of solid foods in infants: results of a cohort study. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9:60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Olson BH, Horodynski MA, Brophy-Herb H, Iwanski KC. Health professionals’ perspectives on the infant feeding practices of low income mothers. Matern Child Health J. 2010;14(1):75–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Yu Z, Han S, Zhu J, Sun X, Ji C, Guo X. Pre-pregnancy body mass index in relation to infant birth weight and offspring overweight/obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e61627.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Reynolds RM. Childhood obesity: the impact of maternal obesity on childhood obesity. In: Ovesen PG, Jensen DM, editors. Maternal obesity and pregnancy. Berlin: Springer; 2012. p. 255–70.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  33. Oken E, Levitan EB, Gillman MW. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child overweight: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Obes. 2008;32(2):201–10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Rayfield S, Plugge E. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between maternal smoking in pregnancy and childhood overweight and obesity. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2017;71(2):162–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Ino T. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring obesity: meta-analysis. Pediatr Int Off J Japan Pediatr Soc. 2010;52(1):94–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Lamb MM, Dabelea D, Yin X, Ogden LG, Klingensmith GJ, Rewers M, et al. Early-life predictors of higher body mass index in healthy children. Ann Nutr Metab. 2010;56(1):16–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Diderichsen F, Andersen I, Manuel C, Nybo Andersen A-M, Bach E, Baadsgaard M, et al. Health inequality - determinants and policies. Scand J Public Health. 2012;40(Suppl 8):12–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Karaolis-Danckert N, Buyken AE, Kulig M, Kroke A, Forster J, Kamin W, et al. How pre- and postnatal risk factors modify the effect of rapid weight gain in infancy and early childhood on subsequent fat mass development: results from the multicenter allergy study 90. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(5):1356–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Stettler N, Kumanyika SK, Katz SH, Zemel BS, Stallings VA. Rapid weight gain during infancy and obesity in young adulthood in a cohort of African Americans. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;77(6):1374–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. The Danish Health Authority. Monitorering af vækst hos 0–5-årige børn: Vejledning til sundhedsplejersker og praktiserende læger. Copenhagen; 2015.

  41. Høstgaard AM, Pape-Haugaard L. Reusable data in public health databases-problems encountered in Danish Children’s database. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2012;180:609–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. The Danish Health Data Authority. Dokumentation af registre: Børnedatabasen [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2018 Aug 24]. Available from: https://www.esundhed.dk/Dokumentation/DocumentationExtended?id=20

  43. Schmidt M, Pedersen L, Sørensen HT. The Danish civil registration system as a tool in epidemiology. Eur J Epidemiol. 2014;29(8):541–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Pedersen CB. The Danish civil registration system. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):22–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Knudsen LB, Olsen J. The Danish medical birth registry. Dan Med Bull. 1998;45(3):320–3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Baadsgaard M, Quitzau J. Danish registers on personal income and transfer payments. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):103–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Jensen VM, Rasmussen AW. Danish education registers. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):91–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. World Health Organization. WHO Anthro (version 3.2.2, January 2011) and macros [Internet]. 2011. Available from: http://www.who.int/childgrowth/software/en/

  49. Ong KKL, Ahmed ML, Emmett PM, Preece MA, Dunger DB. The Avon longitudinal study of pregnancy and childhood study team. Association between postnatal catch-up growth and obesity in childhood: prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2000;320(7240):967–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. World Health Organization. WHO Child Growth Standards STATA igrowup package. 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  51. World Health Organization. Training course on child growth assessment. Geneva; 2008.

  52. de Onis M, Lobstein T. Defining obesity risk status in the general childhood population: which cut-offs should we use? Int J Pediatr Obes. 2010;5(6):458–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. UNESCO Institute for Statistics. International Standard Classification of Education ISCED 2011. Montreal, Cannada; 2012.

  54. Ullits LR, Ejlskov L, Mortensen RN, Hansen SM, Kræmer SRJ, Vardinghus-Nielsen H, et al. Socioeconomic inequality and mortality - a regional Danish cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:490.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Horta BL, Victora CG. Long-term effects of breastfeeding: A systematic review. Vol. 74. Geneva; 2013.

  56. Kronborg H, Foverskov E, Væth M. Breastfeeding and introduction of complementary food in Danish infants. Scand J Public Health. 2015;43(2):138–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Kramer MS, Platt RW, Wen SW, Joseph KS, Allen A, Abrahamowicz M, et al. A new and improved population-based Canadian reference for birth weight for gestational age. Pediatrics. 2001;108(2):E35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. World Health Organization. Body mass index - BMI [Internet]. World Health Organization; 2018 [cited 2018 Feb 24]. Available from: http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/disease-prevention/nutrition/a-healthy-lifestyle/body-mass-index-bmi

  59. Buuren S. Van, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw. 2011;45(3):1–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Sterne JAC, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, et al. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009;338:b2393.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. VanderWeele TJ, Knol MJ. A tutorial on interaction. Epidemiol Method. 2014;3(1):33–72.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Knol MJ, VanderWeele TJ, Groenwold RHH, Klungel OH, Rovers MM, Grobbee DE. Estimating measures of interaction on an additive scale for preventive exposures. Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26(6):433–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. R Core Team. R: A language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna; 2018.

  64. The National Committee on Health Research Ethics. Act on research ethics review of health research projects [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2019 Apr 2]. Available from: http://en.nvk.dk/rules-and-guidelines/act-on-research-ethics-review-of-health-research-projects

  65. Solar O, Irwin A. A conceptual framework for action on the social determinants of health. Social Determinants of Health Discussion. Paper 2 (Policy and Practice). Geneva; 2010.

  66. Galobardes B, Shaw M, Lawlor DA, Lynch JW, Davey SG. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1). J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60(1):7–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Yu Z, Han S, Zhu G, Zhu C, Wang X, Cao X, et al. Birth weight and subsequent risk of obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2011;12(7):525–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Schellong K, Schulz S, Harder T, Plagemann A. Birth weight and long-term overweight risk: systematic review and a meta-analysis including 643,902 persons from 66 studies and 26 countries globally. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47776.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Druet C, Stettler N, Sharp S, Simmons RK, Cooper C, Davey Smith G, et al. Prediction of childhood obesity by infancy weight gain: an individual-level meta-analysis. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2012;26(1):19–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Johnson W, Bann D, Hardy R. Infant weight gain and adolescent body mass index: comparison across two British cohorts born in 1946 and 2001. Arch Dis Child. 2018;103(10):974–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Boone-Heinonen J, Messer L, Andrade K, Takemoto E. Connecting the dots in childhood obesity disparities: a review of growth patterns from birth to pre-adolescence. Curr Epidemiol Reports. 2016;3:113–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. White IR, Carlin JB. Bias and efficiency of multiple imputation compared with complete-case analysis for missing covariate values. Stat Med. 2010;29(28):2920–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Knai C, Lobstein T, Darmon N, Rutter H, McKee M. Socioeconomic patterning of childhood overweight status in Europe. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2012;9(4):1472–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Lorenc T, Petticrew M, Welch V, Tugwell P. What types of interventions generate inequalities? Evidence from systematic reviews. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2013;67(2):190–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Whitehead M. A typology of actions to tackle social inequalities in health. J Epidemiol Community Heal. 2007;61(6):473–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable

Funding

The project is funded by Aalborg University, Denmark. The work was undertaken with the support of The Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), a UKCRC Public Health Research Centre of Excellence. Joint funding (MR/KO232331/1) from the British Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, the Welsh Government and the Wellcome Trust, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged. The funding body had no role in the study design, the analysis or interpretation of data, or in the in the writing of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TAR, HB, CO and AMBH conceptualised the paper. TAR, HB, RNM and GJMT performed the analysis. TAR, HB, RNM, GJMT, LRU, CO, CTP and AMBH helped in the interpretation of data. TAR composed the first draft and HB, RNM, GJMT, LRU, CO, CTP and AMBH made substantial contributions in the process of revising the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Torill Alise Rotevatn.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by The Danish Data Protection Agency (ref: 2008-58-0028, internal ref.: 2017–67). In Denmark, it is not necessary to obtain ethical approval for the use of administrative register information in research, as long as the project does not involve human biological material.

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

Henrik Bøggild is a member of the editorial board of this journal. The authors declare that they have no further competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Tables of covariate distribution on maternal education. A table presenting how covariates are distributed across levels of maternal education. (DOCX 21 kb)

Additional file 2:

Tables of covariate distribution on household income. A table presenting how covariates are distributed across levels of household income. (DOCX 21 kb)

Additional file 3:

Results from logistic regression models conducted on the population with complete data. A table showing the results obtained from the logistic regression models when these are based on the population with complete data. (DOCX 22 kb)

Additional file 4:

Results from logistic regression models conducted on the population with complete data, stratified by maternal education or household income. A table showing the results obtained from the logistic regression models when these are based on the population with complete data and stratified by maternal education or household income, respectively. (DOCX 14 kb)

Additional file 5:

Comparison of populations with and without missing data on exposure and outcome. A table showing comparing populations with and without missing data on exposure and outcome on central variables. (DOCX 17 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rotevatn, T.A., Overgaard, C., Melendez-Torres, G.J. et al. Infancy weight gain, parental socioeconomic position, and childhood overweight and obesity: a Danish register-based cohort study. BMC Public Health 19, 1209 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-019-7537-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-019-7537-z

Keywords